Advertisement

Are two spaces better than one? The effect of spacing following periods and commas during reading

  • Rebecca L. Johnson
  • Becky Bui
  • Lindsay L. Schmitt
Article

Abstract

The most recent edition of the American Psychological Association (APA) Manual states that two spaces should follow the punctuation at the end of a sentence. This is in contrast to the one-space requirement from previous editions. However, to date, there has been no empirical support for either convention. In the current study, participants performed (1) a typing task to assess spacing usage and (2) an eye-tracking experiment to assess the effect that punctuation spacing has on reading performance. Although comprehension was not affected by punctuation spacing, the eye movement record suggested that initial processing of the text was facilitated when periods were followed by two spaces, supporting the change made to the APA Manual. Individuals’ typing usage also influenced these effects such that those who use two spaces following a period showed the greatest overall facilitation from reading with two spaces.

Keywords

eye-tracking reading punctuation spacing 

Notes

Acknowledgements

The order of authorship for the second and third authors was determined by a coin-toss. We would like to thank Tom Allen, Angela Persico, and Alexa Banculli for their assistance in data collection and analysis and Denis Drieghe for his helpful comments on a previous version of this manuscript. This research was funded in part with a Summer Collaborative Research Grant at Skidmore College. This work was previously presented at the 83rd Annual Meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association in Pittsburgh, PA. Correspondence concerning the paper should be addressed to Dr. Rebecca Johnson, Department of Psychology, Skidmore College, Saratoga Springs, NY 12866, USA. (email: rjohnso1@skidmore.edu).

References

  1. American Psychological Association. (2001, Fifth Edition). Publication manual of the American Psychological Association. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  2. American Psychological Association. (2010, Sixth Edition). Publication manual of the American Psychological Association. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  3. Cui, L., Drieghe, D., Bai, X., Yan, G., & Liversedge, S. P. (2014). Parafoveal preview benefit in unspaced and spaced Chinese reading. The Quarterly Journal Of Experimental Psychology, 67(11), 2172-2188.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2014.909858 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Drieghe, D., Brysbaert, M., & Desmet, T. (2005). Parafoveal-on-foveal effects on eye movements in text reading: Does an extra space make a difference? Vision Research, 45, 1693-1706.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2005.01.010 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Drieghe, D., Fitzsimmons, G., & Liversedge, S. (2017). Parafoveal preview effects in reading unspaced text. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(10), 1701-1716.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Epelboim, J., Booth, J. R., Ashkenazy, R., Taleghani, A., & Steinman, R. M. (1997). Fillers and spaces in text: Evidence for the importance of word recognition during reading. Vision Research, 37, 2899–2914.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(97)00095-3 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Gonzalez, J. (August, 2014). Nothing says over 40 like two spaces after a period! [Web log message]. Retrieved from https://www.cultofpedagogy.com/two-spaces-after-period/
  8. Grainger, J., Tydgat, I., & Issele, J. (2010). Crowding affects letters and symbols differently. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 673-688.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016888 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Hill, R. L., & Murray, W. S. (2000). Commas and spaces: Effects of punctuation on eye movements and sentence parsing. In A. Kennedy, R. Radach, D. Heller, & J. Pynte (Eds.), Reading as a perceptual process (pp. 565–589). Oxford: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Hirotani, M., Frazier, L., & Rayner, K. (2006). Punctuation and intonation effects on clause and sentence wrap-up: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 54, 425–443.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.12.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Jacobs, A. (1987). On the role of blank spaces for eye-movement control in visual search. Perception & Psychophysics, 41(5), 473-479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Johnson, R. L., & Eisler, M. E. (2012). The importance of the first and last letter in words during sentence reading. Acta Psychologica, 141, 336-351.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Bojesen Christensen, R. H. (2015) LmerTest: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models.Google Scholar
  14. Lee, T. (2011, January). Re: Everyone has a right to their beliefs [Web log message]. Retrieved from http://www.manifestdensity.net/2011/01/14/everyone-has-a-right-to-their-beliefs/
  15. Legge, G. E., Mansfield, J. S., & Chung, S. T. L. (2001). Psychophysics of reading XX. Linking letter recognition to reading speed in central and peripheral vision. Vision Research, 41, 725-743.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(00)00295-9 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Levi, D. M. (2008). Crowding: An essential bottleneck for object recognition: A mini-review. Vision Research, 48, 635-654.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2007.12.009 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  17. Malt, B. C., & Seamon, J. G. (1978). Peripheral and cognitive components of eye guidance in filled-space reading. Perception & Psychophysics, 23, 399–402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Manjoo, F. (2011, January). Space invaders: Why you should never, ever use two spaces after a period. Slate Magazine. Retrieved from http://www.slate.com/id/2281146/
  19. McArdle, M. (2011, January). You can have my double space when you pry it from my cold, dead hands. The Atlantic Magazine. Retrieved from http://www.theatlantic.com/entertainment/archive/2011/01/you-can-have-my-double-space-when-you-pry-it-from-my-cold-dead-hands/69592/
  20. McConkie, G. W., & Rayner, K. (1975). The span of the effective stimulus during a fixation in reading. Perception & Psychophysics, 17(6), 578-586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. McGowan, V. A., White, S. J., Jordan, T. R., & Paterson, K. B. (2014). Aging and the use of interword spaces during reading: Evidence from eye movements. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(3), 740-747.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0527-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. McGowan, V. A., White, S. J., & Paterson, K. B. (2015). The effects of interword spacing on the eye movements of young and older readers. Journal Of Cognitive Psychology, 27(5), 609-621.  https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2014.988157 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Morris, R. K., Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (1990). Eye movement guidance in reading: The role of parafoveal letter and space information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 268–281.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.16.2.268 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Paterson, K. B., & Jordan, T. R. (2010). Effects of increased letter spacing on word identification and eye guidance during reading. Memory & Cognition, 38(4), 502-512.  https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.38.4.502 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Payne, B. R., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. L. (2012). Aging, parafoveal preview, and semantic integration in sentence processing: Testing the cognitive workload of wrap-up. Psychology and Aging, 27, 638-649.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026540 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  26. Pelli, D., Tillman, K. A., Freeman, J., Su, M., Berger, T. D., & Majaj, N. J. (2007). Crowding and eccentricity determine reading rate. Journal of Vision, 7, 1-36.  https://doi.org/10.1167/7.2.20 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Perea, M., & Acha, J. (2009). Space information is important for reading. Vision Research, 49, 1994-2000.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2009.05.009 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Perea, M., Moret-Tatay, C., & Gómez, P. (2011). The effects of interletter spacing in visual-word recognition. Acta Psychologica, 137(3), 345-351.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.04.003 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Pollatsek, A., & Rayner, K. (1982). Eye movement control in reading: The role of word boundaries. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 8(6), 817-833.Google Scholar
  30. Pynte, J., & Kennedy, A. (2007). The influence of punctuation and word class on distributed processing in normal reading. Vision Research, 47, 1215-1227.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2006.12.00 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 372-422.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.124.3.372 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Rayner, K. (2009). The Thirty Fifth Sir Frederick Bartlett lecture: Eye movements and attention during reading, scene perception, and visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 1457-1506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Rayner, K., Fischer, M. H., & Pollatsek, A. (1998). Unspaced text interferes with both word identification and eye movement control. Vision Research, 38(8), 1129-1144.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Rayner, K., Kambe, G., & Duffy, S. A. (2000). Clause wrap-up effects on eye movements during reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53, 1061–1080.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Rayner, K., McConkie, G. W., & Zola, D. (1980). Integrating information across eye movements. Cognitive Psychology, 12(2), 206-226.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(80)90009-2 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Rayner, K., Yang, J., Schuett, S., & Slattery, T. J. (2013). Eye movements of older and younger readers when reading unspaced text. Experimental Psychology, 60, 354-361.  https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000207 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Schotter, E. R., Angele, B., & Rayner, K. (2012). Parafoveal processing in reading. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 74, 5-35.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-011-0219-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Slattery, T. J., Yates, M., & Angele, B. (2016). Interword and interletter spacing effects during reading revisited: Interactions with word and font characteristics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 22, 406-422.  https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000104 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Spragins, A. B., Lefton, L. A., & Fischer, D. F. (1976). Eye movements while reading and searching spatially transformed text: A developmental examination. Memory & Cognition, 4(1), 36-42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. van den Boer, M., & Hakvoort, B. E. (2015). Default spacing is the optimal spacing for word reading. The Quarterly Journal Of Experimental Psychology, 68(4), 697-709.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2014.964272 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Van Overshelde, J. P., & Healy, A. F. (2005). A blank look in reading: The effect of blank space on the identification of letters and words during reading. Experimental Psychology, 52(3), 213-223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Walker, A. (2011, January). GOOD Design Daily: Do you double space after periods? The Daily GOOD Magazine. Retrieved from http://www.good.is/post/good-design-daily-do-you-double-space-after-periods/
  43. Warren, T., White, S., & Reichle, E. (2009). Investigating the causes of wrap-up effects: Evidence from eye movements and E-Z Reader. Cognition, 111, 132-137.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.12.011 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  44. White, S. J., Warren, T. & Reichle, E. D. (2011). Parafoveal preview during reading: Effects of sentence position. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1221-1238.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022190 PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  45. Wiederkehr, S. (2009, July 30). On two spaces following a period [Web log message]. Retrieved from http://blog.apastyle.org/apastyle/2009/07/on-two-spaces-following-a-period.html
  46. Wolford, G., & Chambers, L. (1983). Lateral masking as a function of spacing. Perception & Psychophysics, 33, 129-138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Yu, D., Cheung, S. H., Legge, G. E., & Chung, S. T. L. (2007). Effect of letter spacing on visual span and reading speed. Journal of Vision, 7(2), 1-10.  https://doi.org/10.1167/7.2.2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Zorzi, M., Barbiero, C., Facoetti, A., Lonciari, I., Carrozzi, M., Montico, M.,… Ziegler, J. C. (2012). Extra-large spacing improves reading in dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109, 11455-11459.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1205566109 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Rebecca L. Johnson
    • 1
  • Becky Bui
    • 1
  • Lindsay L. Schmitt
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologySkidmore CollegeSaratoga SpringsUSA

Personalised recommendations