Advertisement

Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics

, Volume 80, Issue 6, pp 1584–1590 | Cite as

Perception of time to contact of slow- and fast-moving objects using monocular and binocular motion information

  • Aaron J. Fath
  • Mats Lind
  • Geoffrey P. Bingham
Article

Abstract

The role of the monocular-flow-based optical variable τ in the perception of the time to contact of approaching objects has been well-studied. There are additional contributions from binocular sources of information, such as changes in disparity over time (CDOT), but these are less understood. We conducted an experiment to determine whether an object’s velocity affects which source is most effective for perceiving time to contact. We presented participants with stimuli that simulated two approaching squares. During approach the squares disappeared, and participants indicated which square would have contacted them first. Approach was specified by (a) only disparity-based information, (b) only monocular flow, or (c) all sources of information in normal viewing conditions. As expected, participants were more accurate at judging fast objects when only monocular flow was available than when only CDOT was. In contrast, participants were more accurate judging slow objects with only CDOT than with only monocular flow. For both ranges of velocity, the condition with both information sources yielded performance equivalent to the better of the single-source conditions. These results show that different sources of motion information are used to perceive time to contact and play different roles in allowing for stable perception across a variety of conditions.

Keywords

Time to contact Stereomotion Binocular vision Motion perception Binocular disparity CDOT Optic flow 

References

  1. Anderson, J., & Bingham, G. P. (2010). A solution to the online guidance problem for targeted reaches: Proportional rate control using relative disparity τ. Experimental Brain Research, 205, 291–306.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-010-2361-9 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Anderson, J., & Bingham, G. P. (2011). Locomoting-to-reach: Information variables and control strategies for nested actions. Experimental Brain Research, 214, 631–644.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-011-2865-y CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433–436.  https://doi.org/10.1163/156856897X00357 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. DeLucia, P. R. (2005). Does binocular disparity or familiar size information override effects of relative size on judgements of time to contact? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 865–886.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980443000377 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. DeLucia, P. R., Kaiser, M. K., Bush, J. M., Meyer, L. E., & Sweet, B. T. (2003). Information integration in judgements of time to contact. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 1165−1189.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980343000053 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Fath, A. J., Marks, B. S., Snapp-Childs, W., & Bingham, G. P. (2014). Information and control strategy to solve the degrees-of-freedom problem for nested locomotion-to-reach. Experimental Brain Research, 232, 3821–3831.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Gray, R., & Regan, D. (1998). Accuracy of estimating time to collision using binocular and monocular information. Vision Research, 38, 499–512.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(97)00230-7 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Gray, R., & Regan, D. (2000). Estimating the time to collision with a rotating nonspherical object. Vision Research, 40, 49–63.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(99)00157-1 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Gray, R., & Regan, D. (2004). The use of binocular time to contact information. In H. Hecht & G. J. P. Savelsbergh (Eds.), Advances in psychology: Vol. 135. Theories of time-to-contact (pp. 303–325). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Harris, J. M., Nefs, H. T., & Grafton, C. E. (2008). Binocular vision and motion-in-depth. Spatial Vision, 21, 531–547.  https://doi.org/10.1163/156856808786451462 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Harris, J. M., & Watamaniuk, S. N. (1995). Speed discrimination of motion-in-depth using binocular cues, Vision Research, 35, 885–896.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Heuer, H. (1993). Estimates of time to contact based on changing size and changing target vergence. Perception, 22, 549–563.  https://doi.org/10.1068/p220549 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Kleiner, M., Brainard, D., & Pelli, D. (2007). What’s new in Psychtoolbox-3? Perception, 36 (ECVP Abstract Suppl.), 14.Google Scholar
  14. Lee, D. N. (1976). A theory of visual control of braking based on information about time-to-collision. Perception, 5, 437–459.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Miles, W. R. (1930). Ocular dominance in human adults. Journal of General Psychology, 3, 412–430.  https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.1930.9918218 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Nefs, H.T., O’Hare, L., & Harris, J.M. (2010). Two independent mechanisms for motion-in-depth perception: Evidence from individual differences. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 155.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00155 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  17. Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437–442.  https://doi.org/10.1163/156856897X00366 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Regan, D., & Beverley, K. I. (1973). Some dynamic features of depth perception. Vision Research, 13, 2369–2379.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(73)90236-8 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Rushton, S. K., & Wann, J. P. (1999). Weighted combination of size and disparity: A computational model for timing a ball catch. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 186–190.  https://doi.org/10.1038/5750 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Todd, J. T. (1981). Visual information about moving objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 795–810.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.7.4.795 Google Scholar
  21. Tyler, C. W. (1971). Steroscopic depth movement: Two eyes less sensitive than one. Science, 174, 958–961.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Warren, W. H., Jr. (1998). Visually controlled locomotion: 40 years later. Ecological Psychology, 10, 177-219.  https://doi.org/10.1080/10407413.1998.9652682 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Watt, S. J., & Bradshaw, M. F. (2003). The visual control of reaching and grasping: Binocular disparity and motion parallax. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 404–415.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.29.2.404 PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Aaron J. Fath
    • 1
  • Mats Lind
    • 2
  • Geoffrey P. Bingham
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of Southern MississippiHattiesburgUSA
  2. 2.Department of Information TechnologyUppsala UniversityUppsalaSweden
  3. 3.Department of Psychological and Brain SciencesIndiana UniversityBloomingtonUSA

Personalised recommendations