Advertisement

Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics

, Volume 77, Issue 2, pp 551–559 | Cite as

Listening to music reduces eye movements

  • Thomas SchäferEmail author
  • Jörg Fachner
Article

Abstract

Listening to music can change the way that people visually experience the environment, probably as a result of an inwardly directed shift of attention. We investigated whether this attentional shift can be demonstrated by reduced eye movement activity, and if so, whether that reduction depends on absorption. Participants listened to their preferred music, to unknown neutral music, or to no music while viewing a visual stimulus (a picture or a film clip). Preference and absorption were significantly higher for the preferred music than for the unknown music. Participants exhibited longer fixations, fewer saccades, and more blinks when they listened to music than when they sat in silence. However, no differences emerged between the preferred music condition and the neutral music condition. Thus, music significantly reduces eye movement activity, but an attentional shift from the outer to the inner world (i.e., to the emotions and memories evoked by the music) emerged as only one potential explanation. Other explanations, such as a shift of attention from visual to auditory input, are discussed.

Keywords

Eye movements Music Music preference Absorption Attention 

References

  1. Bailey, N., & Areni, C. S. (2006). When a few minutes sound like a lifetime: Does atmospheric music expand or contract perceived time? Journal of Retailing, 82, 189–202. doi: 10.1016/j.jretai.2006.05.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Beanland, V., Allen, R. A., & Pammer, K. (2011). Attending to music decreases inattentional blindness. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 1282–1292. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.04.009 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Becker, J. (2004). Deep listeners: Music, emotion, and trancing. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
  4. Becker-Blease, K. A. (2004). Dissociative states through new age and electronic trance music. Journal of Trauma and Dissociation, 5, 89–100. doi: 10.1300/J229v05n02_05 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Blood, A. J., & Zatorre, R. J. (2001). Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 11818–11823. doi: 10.1073/pnas.191355898 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Cardena, E. (2005). The phenomenology of deep hypnosis: Quiescent and physically active. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 53, 37–59. doi: 10.1080/00207140490914234 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Cauchard, F., Cane, J. E., & Weger, U. W. (2012). Influence of background speech and music in interrupted reading: An eye-tracking study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26, 381–390. doi: 10.1002/acp.1837 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Chun, M. M., Golomb, J. D., & Turk-Browne, N. B. (2011). A taxonomy of external and internal attention. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 73–101. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100427 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  10. Coutrot, A., Guyader, N., Ionescu, G., & Caplier, A. (2012). Influence of soundtrack on eye movements during video exploration. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 5, 1–10.Google Scholar
  11. Dan, C., Xue, S., Xiaodong, W., Li, Q., & Na, J. (2008). An eye movement research of the influence of music on reading. Psychological Science (China), 31, 385–388.Google Scholar
  12. Dietrich, A. (2003). Functional neuroanatomy of altered states of consciousness: The transient hypofrontality hypothesis. Consciousness and Cognition, 12, 231–256.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Dietrich, A. (2004). Neurocognitive mechanisms underlying the experience of flow. Consciousness and Cognition, 13, 746–761.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Droit-Volet, S., Bigand, E., Ramos, D., & Bueno, J. L. O. (2010). Time flies with music whatever its emotional valence. Acta Psychologica, 135, 226–232.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Fachner, J. (2011a). Drugs, altered states and musical consciousness: Reframing time and space. In E. Clarke & D. Clarke (Eds.), Music and consciousness (pp. 263–280). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Fachner, J. (2011b). Time is the key—Music and ASC. In E. Cardenas, M. Winkelmann, C. Tart, & S. Krippner (Eds.), Altering consciousness: A multidisciplinary perspective. Vol. 1: History, culture and the humanities (pp. 355–376). Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger.Google Scholar
  17. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Gabrielsson, A. (2001). Emotions in strong experiences with music. In P. N. Juslin & J. O. Sloboda (Eds.), Music and emotion (pp. 431–449). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  19. Gabrielsson, A. (2011). Strong experiences with music: Music is much more than just music. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199695225.001.0001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Gabrielsson, A., & Lindström Wik, S. L. (2003). Strong experiences related to music: A descriptive system. Musicae Scientiae, 7, 157–217.Google Scholar
  21. Gromko, J. E. (2004). Predictors of music sight-reading ability in high school wind players. Journal of Research in Music Education, 52, 6–15. doi: 10.2307/3345521 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Herbert, R. (2011). Music listening: Absorption, dissociation and trancing. Aldershot, UK: Ashgate.Google Scholar
  23. Herbert, R. (2012). Musical and non-musical involvement in daily life: The case of absorption. Musicae Scientiae, 16, 41–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Herbert, R. (2013). An empirical study of normative dissociation in musical and non-musical everyday life experiences. Psychology of Music, 41, 372–394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Holmqvist, K., Nyström, M., Andersson, R., Dewhurst, R., Halszka, J., & van de Weijer, J. (2011). Eye tracking: A comprehensive guide to methods and measures. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  26. Huron, D. (2006). Sweet anticipation: Music and the psychology of expectation. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  27. Jamieson, G.A. (2005). The modified Tellegen Absorption Scale: A clearer window on the structure and meaning of absorption. Australian Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 33, 119–139.Google Scholar
  28. Johansson, R., Holmqvist, K., Mossberg, F., & Lindgren, M. (2012). Eye movements and reading comprehension while listening to preferred and non-preferred study music. Psychology of Music, 40, 339–356. doi: 10.1177/0305735610387777 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Juslin, P. N., & Laukka, P. (2003). Communication of emotions in vocal expression and music performance: Different channels, same code? Psychological Bulletin, 129, 770–814. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.5.770 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Juslin, P. N., & Laukka, P. (2004). Expression, perception, and induction of musical emotions: A review and a questionnaire study of everyday listening. Journal of New Music Research, 33, 217–238. doi: 10.1080/0929821042000317813 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Juslin, P. N., & Sloboda, J. A. (2001). Music and emotion. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  32. Kellaris, J. J., & Mantel, S. P. (1996). Shaping time perceptions with background music: The effect of congruity and arousal on estimates of ad durations. Psychology & Marketing, 13, 501–515.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Kowler, E., Anderson, E., Dosher, B., & Blaser, E. (1995). The role of attention in the programming of saccades. Vision Research, 35, 1897–1916. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)00279-U CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Kreutz, G., Ott, U., Teichmann, D., Osawa, P., & Vaitl, D. (2008). Using music to induce emotions: Influences of musical preference and absorption. Psychology of Music, 36, 101–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Lamont, A. (2011). University students’ strong experiences of music: Pleasure, engagement, and meaning. Musicae Scientiae, 15, 229–249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Lavine, R. A., Sibert, J. L., Gokturk, M., & Dickens, B. (2002). Eye-tracking measures and human performance in a vigilance task. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 73, 367–372.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Lopez, L., & Malhotra, R. (1991). Estimation of time intervals with most preferred and least preferred music. Psychological Studies, 36, 203–209.Google Scholar
  38. Moelants, D. (2002). Preferred tempo reconsidered. In J. R. C. Stevens, D. Burnham, G. McPherson, & E. Schubert (Eds.), Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Music Perception and Cognition, Sydney (pp. 580–583). Adelaide, Australia: Causal Productions.Google Scholar
  39. North, A. C., & Hargreaves, D. J. (1995). Subjective complexity, familiarity, and liking for popular music. Psychomusicology, 14, 77–93. doi: 10.1037/h0094090 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. North, A. C., & Hargreaves, D. J. (2008). The social and applied psychology of music. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Oh, J., Han, M., Peterson, B. S., & Jeong, J. (2012). Spontaneous eyeblinks are correlated with responses during the Stroop task. PLoS ONE, 7, e34871. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0034871 CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 160–174. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.109.2.160 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 372–422. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.124.3.372 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Rhodes, L. A., David, D. C., & Combs, A. L. (1988). Absorption and enjoyment of music. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 66, 737–738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Rouget, G. (1985). Music and trance: A theory of the relations between music and possession. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  46. Schäfer, T., Fachner, J., & Smukalla, M. (2013a). Changes in the representation of space and time while listening to music. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(508), 1–15. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00508 Google Scholar
  47. Schäfer, T., & Sedlmeier, P. (2011). Does the body move the soul? The impact of arousal on music preference. Music Perception, 29, 37–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Schäfer, T., Sedlmeier, P., Städtler, C., & Huron, D. (2013b). The psychological functions of music listening. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(511), 1–33. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00511 Google Scholar
  49. Schäfer, T., Smukalla, M., & Oelker, S.-A. (2013c). How music changes our lives. A qualitative study of the long-term effects of intense musical experiences. Psychology of Music, 42, 525–544. doi: 10.1177/0305735613482024 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Schleicher, R., Galley, N., Briest, S., & Galley, L. (2008). Blinks and saccades as indicators of fatigue in sleepiness warnings: Looking tired? Ergonomics, 51, 982–1010.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Smith, C. N., Hopkins, R. O., & Squire, L. R. (2006). Experience-dependent eye movements, awareness, and hippocampus-dependent memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 11304–11312.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Smith, D. T., Schenk, T., & Rorden, C. (2012). Saccade preparation is required for exogenous attention but not endogenous attention or IOR. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 1438–1447. doi: 10.1037/a0027794 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Stern, J. A., Walrath, L. C., & Goldstein, R. (1984). The endogenous eyeblink. Psychophysiology, 21, 22–33. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1984.tb02312.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Stuyven, E., Van der Goten, K., Vandierendonck, A., Claeys, K., & Crevits, L. (2000). The effect of cognitive load on saccadic eye movements. Acta Psychologica, 104, 69–85. doi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(99)00054-2 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Tart, C. (1971). On being stoned: A psychological study of marijuana intoxication. Oxford, UK: Science & Behaviour Books.Google Scholar
  56. Tellegen, A., & Atkinson, G. (1974). Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences (“absorption”), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 83, 268–277. doi: 10.1037/h0036681 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Vaitl, D., Birbaumer, N., Gruzelier, J., Jamieson, G. A., Kotchoubey, B., Kübler, A., & Weiss, T. (2005). Psychobiology of altered states of consciousness. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 98–127. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.98 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. van Zoest, W., Donk, M., & Theeuwes, J. (2004). The role of stimulus-driven and goal-driven control in saccadic visual selection. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 30, 746–759. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.30.4.749 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Wadlinger, H. A., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2006). Positive mood broadens visual attention to positive stimuli. Motivation and Emotion, 30, 89–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Wolfe, J. M., & Horowitz, T. S. (2004). What attributes guide the deployment of visual attention and how do they do it? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 495–501. doi: 10.1038/nrn1411 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Zakay, D., & Block, R. A. (1997). Temporal cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 6, 12–16. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep11512604 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Zatorre, R. J., & Peretz, I. (2001). The biological foundations of music. New York, NY: New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
  63. Zingrone, N. L., Alvarado, C. S., & Cardena, E. (2010). Out-of-body experiences and physical body activity and posture: Responses from a survey conducted in Scotland. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 198, 163–165. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181cc0d6d CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Zou, H., Müller, H. J., & Shi, Z. (2012). Non-spatial sounds regulate eye movements and enhance visual search. Journal of Vision, 12(5), 2. doi: 10.1167/12.5.2. 1–18.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyChemnitz University of TechnologyChemnitzGermany
  2. 2.Department of Music and Performing ArtsAnglia Ruskin UniversityCambridgeUK

Personalised recommendations