Updating the spatiotopic representation
Golomb, J. D., L’Heureux, Z. E., & Kanwisher, N. (2014). Feature-binding errors after eye movements and shifts of attention. Psychological Science, 25, 1067–1078. doi:10.1177/0956797614522068
When the eyes move, the positions of objects in the visual stimulus change on the retina but not in the world. Thus, the visual system needs to take the ever-changing retinotopic input and create a relatively stable spatiotopic view of the world. This remapping has been the subject of a substantial body of work over many years. Much of the interest has been in the dynamics of what happens just before, during, and immediately after a saccadic eye movement. Prior work has focused on where things appear to be at different times—as, for example, in Ross, Morrone, and Burr’s (1997) work showing that visual space seemed to be squeezed just before a saccade. In new work, Golomb, L’Heureux, and Kanwisher (2014) were more interested in what is seen than in where it appears to be located.
Consider the following situation. You are fixating at one point and are cued to make a saccade to another point. Either 50 or 500 ms after the saccade lands, four colored patches appear in a square array surrounding the new fixation point. Let’s call the patches A, B, C, and D. Your job is to report on the color of one of them using a color wheel so that the accuracy of your judgment can be measured in degrees around the hue circle. In the spatiotopic condition, you are asked to report on the color of the patch at a particular location in the world. The patches are arranged so that patch A is at that location now and is, thus, the correct color. Patch B is in the same retinotopic location where the cue had been before the saccade. That is, if the cue was below and to the right of fixation before the saccade, patch B is the patch that is now below and to the right of the new fixation. Thus, it would have been the right answer before the saccade but is not the right answer now. Patch C and patch B are the same distance from patch A, and patch D fills out the square array.
The interesting data in this study come from the errors, which come in two forms. You might swap the correct color for the color of another square, or you might get more or less the correct color but bias your setting toward the color of another square. At the brief delay of 50 ms, both types of error are seen. You were supposed to report color A, but sometimes you report color B, swapping in the color of the retinotopic patch. Patches C and D do not seem to have much influence on these errors. The blending errors are perhaps more interesting: Sometimes you report color A, but with a slight but real bias toward B. It is as if the old, fading, presaccadic map and the new, postsaccadic map are both active at the same time and are mixing together. By 500 ms, these effects are gone.
Suppose you are asked to report on the color of a retinotopically defined patch: What is the color of the patch up and to the left of fixation? In this case, there is no systematic interference from other patches. Only the developing spatiotopic representation shows traces of its presaccadic contents.
Interestingly, both swapping and blending errors can be produced without eye movements. If you are asked to shift attention from A to B just before making a color judgment about the attended item, you tend to make swapping errors but not blending errors. If, on the other hand, you are asked to divide attention between A and B and then to report on B after the patches have been removed, the color of A biases the assessment of B, as if the two colors were imperfectly separated during the division of attention.
These results seem to show that using the retinotopic input to update a spatiotopic representation of the world is not just a matter of changing spatial pointers. The featural contents from one representation can get incorrectly mixed with the contents of the other, at least for a few tens of milliseconds after a saccade. –J.M.W.
Additional references
Ross, J., Morrone, M. C., & Burr, D. C. (1997). Compression of visual space before saccades. Nature, 386, 598–601. doi:10.1038/386598a0