Arias, D., & Otto, C. (2011). Defining the scope of sensory deprivation for long duration space missions (NASA Technical Publication, JSC-CN-24469). Houston, TX: NASA Johnson Space Center.
Google Scholar
Baird, J. C., & Biersdorf, W. R. (1967). Quantitative functions for size and distance judgments. Perception & Psychophysics, 2, 161–166.
Article
Google Scholar
Bengston, J. K., Stergios, J. C., Ward, J. L., & Jester, R. E. (1980). Optic array determinants of apparent distance and size in pictures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 6, 751–759.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Benke, T., Koserenko, O., Watson, N. V., & Gerstenbrand, F. (1993). Space and cognition: The measurement of behavioral functions during a 6-day space mission. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 64, 376–379.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Clément, G., Arnesen, T. N., Olsen, M. H., & Sylvestre, B. (2007). Perception of longitudinal body axis in microgravity during parabolic flight. Neuroscience Letters, 413, 150–153.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Clément, G., Lathan, C. E., & Lockerd, A. (2008). Perception of depth in microgravity during parabolic flight. Acta Astronautica, 63, 828–832.
Article
Google Scholar
Clément, G., & Reschke, M. F. (2008). Neuroscience in space. New York, NY: Springer.
Book
Google Scholar
Craven, B. J. (1993). Orientation dependence of human line-length judgments matches statistical structure in real-world scenes. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 253, 101–106.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
de Schonen, S., Léone, G., & Lipshits, M. (1998). The face inversion effect in microgravity: Is gravity used as a spatial reference for complex object recognition? Acta Astronautica, 42, 287–301.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fineman, M. B., & Carlson, J. (1973). A comparison of the Ponzo illusion with a textual analogue. Perception & Psychophysics, 14, 31–33.
Article
Google Scholar
Ghuman, A. S., McDaniel, J. R., & Martin, A. (2010). Face adaptation without a face. Current Biology, 20, 32–36.
PubMed Central
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Gilinsky, A. S. (1951). Perceived size and distance in visual space. Psychological Review, 58, 460–482.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Glasauer, S., & Mittelstaedt, H. (1998). Perception of spatial orientation in microgravity. Brain Research Reviews, 28, 185–193.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kilbride, P. L., & Leibowitz, H. W. (1975). Factors affecting the magnitude of the Ponzo perspective illusion among the Baganda. Perception & Psychophysics, 17, 543–548.
Article
Google Scholar
Koenderink, J. J., van Doorn, A. J., & Wagemans, J. (2011). Depth. i-Perception, 2, 541–564. doi:10.1068/i0438aap
PubMed Central
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kraft, R. N., Patterson, J. E., & Mitchell, N. B. (1986). Distance perception in photographic displays of natural settings. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 62, 179–186.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Lappin, J. S., Shelton, A. L., & Rieser, J. J. (2006). Environmental context influences visually perceived distance. Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 571–581. doi:10.3758/BF03208759
Article
Google Scholar
Lathan, C., Wang, Z. Y., & Clement, G. (2000). Changes in the vertical size of a three-dimensional object drawn in weightlessness by astronauts. Neuroscience Letters, 295, 37–40.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Léone, G. (1998). The effect of gravity on human recognition of disoriented objects. Brain Research Reviews, 28, 203–214.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Li, C.-Y., & Guo, K. (1995). Measurements of geometric illusions, illusory contours and stereo-depth at luminance and colour contrast. Vision Research, 35, 1713–1720. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(94)00212-5
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Loomis, J. M., Da Silva, J. A., Fujita, N., & Fukusima, S. S. (1992). Visual space perception and visually directed action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 906–921. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.18.4.906
PubMed
Google Scholar
Loomis, J. M., & Philbeck, J. W. (1999). Is the anisotropy of perceived 3-D shape invariant across scale? Perception & Psychophysics, 61, 397–402.
Article
Google Scholar
Loomis, J. M., Philbeck, J. W., & Zahorik, P. (2002). Dissociation between location and shape in visual space. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1202–1212. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.28.5.1202
PubMed
Google Scholar
Mamassian, P., & de Montalembert, M. (2010). A simple model of the vertical-horizontal illusion. Vision Research, 50, 956–962.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Norman, J. F., Todd, J. T., Perotti, V. J., & Tittle, J. S. (1996). The visual perception of three-dimensional length. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 173–186. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.22.1.173
PubMed
Google Scholar
O’Shea, R. P., & Ross, H. E. (2007). Judgments of visually perceived eye level (VPEL) in outdoor scenes: Effect of slope and height. Perception, 36, 1168–1178.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ohnaka, Y., & Matsuda, T. (2008). Effects of aspect-ratio and size of photographs upon the depth impression and the depth perception on their scenes. Ritsumeikan Journal of Human Sciences, 17, 15–24.
Google Scholar
Oman, C. M. (2007). Spatial orientation and navigation in microgravity. In F. Mast & L. Jäncke (Eds.), Spatial processing in navigation, imagery and perception (pp. 209–247). New York, NY: Springer.
Chapter
Google Scholar
Ooi, T. L., Wu, B., & He, Z. J. (2001). Distance determined by the angular declination below the horizon. Nature, 414, 197–200. doi:10.1038/35102562
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Prinzmetal, W., Shimamura, A. P., & Mikolinski, M. (2001). The Ponzo illusion and the perception of orientation. Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 99–114.
Article
Google Scholar
Rand, K., Tarampi, M. R., Creem-Regehr, S. H., & Thompson, W. B. (2012). The influence of ground contact and visible horizon on perception of distance and size under severely degraded vision. Seeing and Perceiving, 25, 425–447.
PubMed Central
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Rensink, R. A., O’Regan, J. K., & Clark, J. J. (1997). To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychological Science, 8, 368–373. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00427.x
Article
Google Scholar
Scholl, B. J. (2000). Attenuated change blindness for exogenously attended items in a flicker paradigm. Visual Cognition, 7, 377–396. doi:10.1080/135062800394856
Article
Google Scholar
Sedgwick, H. A. (1986). Space perception. In K. R. Boff, L. Kaufman, & J. P. Thomas (Eds.), Handbook of perception and human performance (pp. 21.1–21.57). New York, NY: Wiley.
Google Scholar
Simons, D. J., & Rensink, R. A. (2005). Change blindness: Past, present, and future. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 16–20. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.11.006
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Smith, W. H. (1966). Observations over the lifetime of a small isolated group: Structure, danger, boredom and vision. Psychological Reports, 19, 475–514.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Stefanucci, J. K., Lessard, D., Geuss, M., Creem-Regehr, S. H., & Thompson, W. B. (2012, August). Evaluating the accuracy of size perception in real and virtual environments. Paper presented at the 9th Annual ACM Symposium on Applied Perception, Los Angeles, CA.
Trommershäuser, J., Maloney, L. T., & Landy, M. S. (2003). The consistency of bisection judgments in visual grasp space. Journal of Vision, 3(11):13, 795–807. doi:10.1167/3.11.13
Google Scholar
Villard, E., Garcia-Moreno, F. T., Peter, N., & Clément, G. (2005). Geometric visual illusions in microgravity during parabolic flight. NeuroReport, 16, 1395–1398.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Watanabe, T. (2006). Geometrical structures of photographic and stereoscopic spaces. Spanish Journal of Psychology, 9, 263–272.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Zubek, J. P. (Ed.). (1969). Sensory deprivation: Fifteen years of research. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Google Scholar