Adams, P. A. (1954). The effect of past experience on the perspective reversal of a tridimensional figure. American Journal of Psychology, 67, 708–710.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Brainard, D. H. (1997). The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433–436. doi:10.1163/156856897X00357
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Brascamp, J. W., Kanai, R., Walsh, V., & van Ee, R. (2010). Human middle temporal cortex, perceptual bias, and perceptual memory for ambiguous three-dimensional motion. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 760–766. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4171-09.2010
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Brascamp, J. W., Knapen, T. H. J., Kanai, R., Noest, A. J., van Ee, R., & van den Berg, A. V. (2008). Multi-timescale perceptual history resolves visual ambiguity. PLoS ONE, 3, e1497. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0001497
PubMed Central
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Brascamp, J. W., Knapen, T. H. J., Kanai, R., van Ee, R., & van den Berg, A. V. (2007). Flash suppression and flash facilitation in binocular rivalry. Journal of Vision, 7(12), 12. doi:10.1167/7.12.12. 1–12.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Carter, O., & Cavanagh, P. (2007). Onset rivalry: Brief presentation isolates an early independent phase of perceptual competition. PLoS ONE, 2, e343. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000343
PubMed Central
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Chen, X., & He, S. (2004). Local factors determine the stabilization of monocular ambiguous and binocular rivalry stimuli. Current Biology, 14, 1013–1017. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2004.05.042
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Gigante, G., Mattia, M., Braun, J., & Del Giudice, P. (2009). Bistable perception modeled as competing stochastic integrations at two levels. PLoS Computational Biology, 5, e1000430. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000430
PubMed Central
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Husain, M., Treue, S., & Andersen, R. A. (1989). Surface interpolation in three-dimensional structure-from-motion perception. Neural Computation, 1, 324–333. doi:10.1162/neco.1989.1.3.324
Article
Google Scholar
Leopold, D. A., & Logothetis, N. K. N. (1999). Multistable phenomena: Changing views in perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 254–264. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01332-7
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Leopold, D. A., Wilke, M., Maier, A., & Logothetis, N. K. (2002). Stable perception of visually ambiguous patterns. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 605–9. doi:10.1038/nn851
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Maier, A., Wilke, M., Logothetis, N. K., & Leopold, D. A. (2003). Perception of temporally interleaved ambiguous patterns. Current Biology, 13, 1076–1085. doi:10.1016/S0960-9822(03)00414-7
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Malach, R. (2012). Targeting the functional properties of cortical neurons using fMR-adaptation. NeuroImage, 62, 1163–9. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.002
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
McCollough, C. (1965). Color adaptation of edge-detectors in the human visual system. Science, 149, 1115–1116. doi:10.1126/science.149.3688.1115
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Meng, M., & Tong, F. (2004). Can attention selectively bias bistable perception? Differences between binocular rivalry and ambiguous figures. Journal of Vision, 4(7), 2. doi:10:1167/4.7.2. 539–551.
Article
Google Scholar
Mysore, S. G., Vogels, R., Raiguel, S. E., Todd, J. T., & Orban, G. A. (2010). The selectivity of neurons in the macaque fundus of the superior temporal area for three-dimensional structure from motion. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 15491–15508. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0820-10.2010
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Nawrot, M., & Blake, R. (1989). Neural integration of information specifying structure from stereopsis and motion. Science, 244, 716–718. doi:10.1126/science.2717948
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Necker, L. A. (1832). Observations on some remarkable phenomena seen in Switzerland; and an optical phenomenon which occurs on viewing of a crystal or geometrical solid. Philosophical Magazine, 1, 329–337.
Google Scholar
Noest, A. J., van Ee, R., Nijs, M. M., & van Wezel, R. J. A. (2007). Percept-choice sequences driven by interrupted ambiguous stimuli: A low-level neural model. Journal of Vision, 7(8), 10. doi:10.1167/7.8.10
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Noest, A. J., & van Wezel, R. J. A. (2012). Dynamics of temporally interleaved percept-choice sequences: Interaction via adaptation in shared neural populations. Journal of Computational Neuroscience, 32, 177–195. doi:10.1007/s10827-011-0347-7
PubMed Central
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Orbach, J., Ehrlich, D., & Heath, H. A. (1963). Reversibility of the Necker cube. I. An examination of the concept of “satiation of orientation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 17, 439–458. doi:10.2466/pms.1963.17.2.439
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Orban, G. A. (2011). The extraction of 3D shape in the visual system of human and nonhuman primates. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 34, 361–388. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113819
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Pastukhov, A., & Braun, J. (2008). A short-term memory of multi-stable perception. Journal of Vision, 8(13), 7. doi:10.1167/8.13.7. 1–14.
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Pastukhov, A., & Braun, J. (2013a). Disparate time-courses of adaptation and facilitation in multi-stable perception. Learning & Perception, 5, 101–118. doi:10.1556/LP.5.2013.Suppl2.7
Article
Google Scholar
Pastukhov, A., & Braun, J. (2013b). Structure-from-motion: Dissociating perception, neural persistence, and sensory memory of illusory depth and illusory rotation. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 322–340. doi:10.3758/s13414-012-0390-0
Article
Google Scholar
Pastukhov, A., Füllekrug, J., & Braun, J. (2013a). Sensory memory of structure-from-motion is shape-specific. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 1215–1229. doi:10.3758/s13414-013-0471-8
Article
Google Scholar
Pastukhov, A., Lissner, A., & Braun, J. (2013). Perceptual adaptation to structure-from-motion depends on volume, not shape. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Pastukhov, A., Vonau, V., & Braun, J. (2012). Believable change: Bistable reversals are governed by physical plausibility. Journal of Vision, 12(1), 17. doi:10.1167/12.1.17
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Pearson, J., & Brascamp, J. W. (2008). Sensory memory for ambiguous vision. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 334–341. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.05.006
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Pearson, J., & Clifford, C. G. W. (2004). Determinants of visual awareness following interruptions during rivalry. Journal of Vision, 4, 196–202. doi:10:1167/4.3.6
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S., & Anstis, S. M. (1983). Perceptual organization in moving patterns. Nature, 304, 529–531. doi:10.1038/304529a0
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Ross, T. D. (2003). Accurate confidence intervals for binomial proportion and Poisson rate estimation. Computers in Biology and Medicine, 33, 509–531. doi:10.1016/S0010-4825(03)00019-2
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Wig, G. S., & Stevens, W. D. (2007). Reductions in cortical activity during priming. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 17, 171–176. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2007.02.001
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Schwiedrzik, C. M., Ruff, C. C., Lazar, A., Leitner, F. C., Singer, W., & Melloni, L. (2013). Untangling perceptual memory: Hysteresis and adaptation map into separate cortical networks. Cerebral Cortex. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhs396
Google Scholar
Sperling, G., & Dosher, B. A. (1994). Depth from motion. In T. V. Papathomas, A. G. C. Chubb, & E. Kowler (Eds.), Early vision and beyond (pp. 133–142). Cambridge: MIT Press.
Google Scholar
Sterzer, P., & Rees, G. (2008). A neural basis for percept stabilization in binocular rivalry. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 389–399. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20039
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Stonkute, S., Braun, J., & Pastukhov, A. (2012). The role of attention in ambiguous reversals of structure-from-motion. PLoS ONE, 7, e37734. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0037734
PubMed Central
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Tong, F., Meng, M., & Blake, R. (2006). Neural bases of binocular rivalry. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 502–11. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.09.003
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Treue, S., Andersen, R. A., Ando, H., & Hildreth, E. C. (1995). Structure-from-motion: Perceptual evidence for surface interpolation. Vision Research, 35, 139–148. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(94)E0069-W
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Uomori, K., & Nishida, S. (1994). The dynamics of the visual system in combining conflicting KDE and binocular stereopsis cues. Perception & Psychophysics, 55, 526–536. doi:10.3758/BF03205310
Article
Google Scholar
van Ee, R., van Dam, L. C. J., & Brouwer, G. J. (2005). Voluntary control and the dynamics of perceptual bi-stability. Vision Research, 45, 41–55. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2004.07.030
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
von Helmholtz, H. (1925). Treatise on physiological optics (J. P. C. Southhall, Trans.). Menasha: George Banta. Original work published 1866.
Google Scholar
Wallach, H., & O’Connell, D. N. (1953). The kinetic depth effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45, 205–217. doi:10.1037/h0056880
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Ware, C., & Mitchell, D. E. (1974). The spatial selectivity of the tilt aftereffect. Vision Research, 14, 735–737. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(74)90072-8
PubMed
Article
Google Scholar
Wheatstone, C. (1838). Contributions to the physiology of vision—Part the first. On some remarkable, and hitherto unobserved, phenomena of binocular vision. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 128, 371–394.
Google Scholar