Figure 4 shows how well the behavior of the PCT model compared to that of a human participant during a segment of one trial in a compensatory tracking task. The figure shows the cursor movements made by a human participant (labeled Human) and the PCT model controlling p
1 (the cursor–target distance, labeled Distance Control) and p
2 (cursor–target angle, labeled Angle Control) during a 15-s segment of a pursuit-tracking task. The horizontal distance between target and cursor (s) during this trial was 980 pixels (20 cm). The figure also shows movements of the target (labeled Target) during this segment of the task.
It is clear from Fig. 4 that the behavior (cursor movements) of both the distance control and angle control models closely approximates that of the human. The next step in the TCV is to determine which model provides a better fit to the human data. Because the models make different predictions about the effect of variations in horizontal target–cursor separation, s, on behavior, it is possible to determine which model is best by comparing the behavior of the models to that of the human at different values of s. Since the models control different perceptions, the model that gives the best account of the data can be considered to be controlling a perceptual variable that is most like the one controlled by the humans.
The models were tested by having the computer version of each model (the code in Eq. 6) perform the same tracking task as the human participants; the computer tracked the same target movements at the same horizontal separations between target and cursor, s, as did the human participants. The performance of both human participants and the models was measured as the ability to control the vertical distance between cursor and target, keeping it close to zero. The measure of control used was the ratio of the observed variance in target–cursor deviation, var(t − c), to the variance of the target, var(t), which can be considered the expected variance of t − c if the participant did nothing (so that c was constant). If control is good, var(t − c) will be very small relative to var(t), so the ratio var(t − c)/var(t) will be very small. The negative log of this ratio is taken, so that the better the control [i.e., the smaller the ratio var(t − c)/var(t)], the larger the number representing the quality of control.
Tests based on performance measures
The behavior of the distance and angle control models was fit to the human performance data by adjusting the slowing and gain parameters to get the closest fit of the model to human performance at each separation. Figure 5 shows measures of performance at different horizontal separations, s, of cursor and target for the two human participants (R.M., M.T.), as well as for the best-fitting versions of the distance and angle control models. The human performance results are shown as solid black diamonds. The performance of both humans declines as s increases. This decline is captured by the angle control model (solid blue triangles), but not by the distance control model (solid red squares). Indeed, the performance of the distance control model is nearly the same at all values of s for both participants. The performance of the distance control model even increases slightly for the trials performed by participant R.M.
The fit of the models to the human performance data can be measured in terms of the squared correlation, R
2, between human and model performance at each separation, s. The R
2 for the fit of the distance control model to the human performance, averaged over M.T. and R.M., was .51, whereas the average R
2 for the fit of the angle control model to the human performance was .99. Clearly, the angle control model fits the human performance data much better than does the distance control model in terms of the decline in tracking performance with increasing s.
The decline in performance for the angle control model results from the fact that s is included in the calculation of the controlled variable, arctan[(t − c)/s]. Increases in s affect the controlled angle variable in a way that reduces the loop gain of the control system. Loop gain is the product of all gain factors around the control loop. In the model described by Eqs. 3–5, the gain factors are k.i (input function), k.o (output function), and k.e (feedback function). So, the loop gain is proportional to the product k.i*k.o*k.e: The higher the loop gain in a control loop, the better the control (in terms of keeping the controlled variable close to the reference, r). If angle is the controlled variable, then k.i is proportional to the derivative of arctan(t − c)/s. So,
$$ k.i=s/\left[{s}^2+{\left(t\hbox{--} c\right)}^2\right]. $$
(7)
Since the distance between target and cursor (t − c) during a tracking trial is typically being kept relatively small, k.i will decrease exponentially as s increases, resulting in the decrease in performance of the angle control model.
Improving the fit of the models to the human performance data
It is possible that the poor fit of the distance control model to the human performance data results from the fact that the perception of vertical target–cursor distance degrades with increasing horizontal separation, s. Thus, it should be possible to improve the fit of the distance control model by degrading the perception of distance with a “threshold” band. This band was placed around the value of the distance perception, t − c, such that only variations in this variable that are outside of the band are perceived. The width of this threshold band increased with increasing s. By appropriate selection of a threshold width for each value of s, it was possible to match the performance of the distance control model to that of the human participants quite well, as can be seen in the plots of the threshold distance model (the open red squares in Fig. 5).
In order to capture the decline in human performance with increasing s, four parameters, representing the width of the threshold band, must be estimated for the threshold distance model. This decline is captured “automatically” by the angle control model through its inclusion of a number proportional to the value of s—the psychological value of s—in the calculation of the controlled angle perception, which reduces the loop gain with increasing s. However, the performance of the angle control model is much better than that of the humans at all values of s, as can be seen by the fact that, for both R.M. and M.T., the plot of the performance of that model as a function of s (solid blue triangles in Fig. 5) runs parallel to but is much higher on the graph than that for the human data.
The angle control model can be made to more closely approximate the human performance by adding low-pass-filtered random noise to the output of the model. The noise amplitude that produced the best fit for the angle control model was 3 % of the output range. This level of noise seems to be of the correct order of magnitude, on the basis of estimates of the magnitude of neural noise levels derived from neurophysiologic measures (Miller & Troyer, 2002; Nakajima et al., 1978). The performance of the angle control model with added noise (the open blue triangles in Fig. 5; Angle Control + Noise) can be seen to fit the human performance data as well as the threshold distance model.
Since the noise level added to the angle control model was the same for all values of s, only one parameter (noise amplitude) was estimated to achieve the fit of the angle control model to the human data, whereas four parameters—the threshold widths at the different horizontal separations, s—were required to get the same fit for the threshold distance model. Also, the distance control model includes no mechanism that explains the increase in threshold width with the increase in s. Therefore, parsimony would seem to recommend a model that controls angle over one that controls distance as giving the best account of the human data in this task.
However, before concluding that angle is the controlled variable in this task, it is possible to make a more detailed comparison of the models by measuring how well they fit the detailed cursor movements made by the human participants on each trial. If the two models were equally good predictors of overall tracking performance, they would be expected to do equally well at accounting for the detailed time variations in human cursor movements (shown in Fig. 4).
Tests based on model fit to detailed cursor movements
Figure 6 shows the average RMS deviations of model from human cursor movements for the two models that gave equally good fits to the human performance data—the threshold distance and angle control + noise models. The fits of the models are shown as a function of the horizontal separation of target from cursor, s. The difference in average RMS deviation of the two models from the time variations in human cursor movements is significant for both M.T. [t(4) = 3.37, p < .011] and R.M. [t(4) = 2.76, p < .025]. The results in Fig. 6 show that the angle control + noise model gives a much better fit to the human data than does the threshold distance model at all values of s, but particularly at larger values of s. This is strong evidence that angle rather than distance is the perception controlled in this tracking task: The hypothesis that distance is the perceptual basis of tracking can be rejected.
A close look at the time traces of the human and model cursor movements suggests why the angle control + noise and threshold distance models account for the performance data equally well (Fig. 5), whereas the angle control + noise model accounts for the detailed human cursor variation data much better than does the threshold distance model (Fig. 6). The observed decrease in the performance of the two models with increasing s, as can be seen in Fig. 5, results from different characteristics of the detailed behavior of each model. The poorer performance of the angle control + noise model with increasing s results from the fact that, as with the human, the variation of model cursor movements around the target increased as s increased, a reflection of the decreased gain of the control model with increasing s. On the other hand, the poorer performance of the threshold distance model with increasing s resulted from the fact that, unlike the human cursor movements, the model cursor movements remained a constant distance from the target, a distance that increased with increasing s.