The development of optic arrays that present only a movement’s kinematic information has proved valuable in delineating the visual information that underpins the perception of biological motion (Shim & Carlton, 1997; Thomas & Jordan, 2001; see Hodges, Williams, Hayes, & Breslin, 2007, for a review). The simplest example of such an array is found in two equidistant bright spots travelling across a formless background. These are perceived as an invisible moving rod. Likewise, four equidistant spots elicit the perception of a moving square (G. Johansson, 1964; G. Johansson & Jansson, 1968). Similarly, when recordings are made of an actor moving with reflective markers on his or her major joints and the contrast settings of the videos are altered such that only the reflectors appear against a dark background, the result is a dynamic point-light display (PLD), a compelling kinematic impression of a human in motion (Dekeyser, Verfaillie, & Vanrie, 2002; G. Johansson, 1973). The swings, translations, and rotations of the extremities about the reflected joints manifest body displacements with kinematic patterns that are so unique that movements performed by particular actors (Cutting & Kozlowski, 1977; Stevenage, Nixon, & Vince, 2000) of either sex (Kozlowski & Cutting, 1977; Mather & Murdoch, 1994) and under a variety of emotional states (Dittrich, Troscianko, Lea, & Morgan, 1996; Sevdalis & Keller, 2012) can be accurately identified by observers.

Importantly, the PLD also provides a means to explore the degree to which a movement’s visible kinematic information discloses its invisible dynamic properties (Bingham, 1987; Gilden & Proffitt, 1989; Runeson & Frykholm, 1981). For instance, PLD research has identified that sufficient information is contained in movement kinematics for observers to discriminate the weights of objects lifted by an actor (Runeson & Frykholm, 1981), even when the actor feigns the weight of the object that is being lifting (Runeson & Frykholm, 1983). Specifically, the actor’s hip angle, the period of time between the actor establishing a purchase and lifting the object, and lifting velocity contribute to the observer’s ability to detect the true weight of a lifted object (Bingham, 1987; Hamilton & Grafton, 2007; Shim & Carlton, 1997). However, the extrinsic visual characteristics of a lifted object also represent sources of information that can influence the perception of the lifting movement (see R. S. Johansson & Cole, 1994, for a review). Object size, for example, is a powerful determinant of perceived object weight (Flanagan, Bowman, & Johansson, 2006; Flanagan & Wing, 1997; Gordon, Westling, Cole, & Johansson, 1993).

Recently, researchers have started to conduct studies to disentangle the relative contributions of movement kinematics and object characteristics to the accurate perception of intrinsic object characteristics, and in turn, of the forces required to act upon those objects. Prominent in this regard are transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) experiments that have highlighted that the somatotopic patterns of primary motor cortex facilitation that occur during the observation of action (Fadiga, Craighero, & Olivier, 2005; Strafella & Paus, 2000) are highly congruent—with a resolution that reflects the level of individual muscles (Alaerts, Swinnen, & Wenderoth, 2009) and the degree of muscular force (Alaerts, Senot, et al., 2010)—to the patterns associated with performing the observed action. Interestingly, through systematic manipulation of participants’ exposure to visual information about movement kinematics and intrinsic object characteristics, Alaerts and colleagues (Alaerts, Swinnen, & Wenderoth, 2010) were able to apply the TMS approach to highlight primary motor cortex patterns that suggest that observers’ perceptions of lifted weight rely more on visual information concerning the lift kinematics than on object characteristics (see also Senot et al., 2011; Shim, Hecht, Lee, Yook, & Kim, 2009).

In the present experiment, we used the PLD method to create stimuli that would further contribute to the line of inquiry concerning the relative impacts of a lifted object’s extrinsic size characteristics and the actor’s movement kinematics to the perception of the observed lifted weight. To do so, participants viewed an actor lifting boxes that varied in both size (small, medium, or large) and weight (25, 50, or 75 lb) across PLD and full-vision video conditions and provided their estimations of the weight of the lifted boxes. Our PLD generation system allowed us to remove point-light markers from the displays systematically, so that we were able to present only the markers associated with the actor and remove those associated with the lifted object, and vice versa. This manipulation allowed us to test the following hypotheses:

  1. 1.

    Participants will be able to differentiate the lifted weights when the movements are presented in degraded arrays that include only the point-light markers associated with the actor (i.e., without interference from the box) (e.g., Runeson & Frykholm, 1983).

  2. 2.

    The addition of kinematic information associated with the lift will improve the differentiation of weight estimates. Specifically, we anticipate better weight differentiation when the observers are presented with the point-light markers associated with the box in motion rather than at rest, and that the differentiation will improve further when the observers are presented with the point-light markers associated with both the box and the actor in motion.

  3. 3.

    Participants will provide estimates of observed weight that reflect the object size when the degraded array presents only the point-light markers associated with the box at rest (i.e., without interference from movement kinematics) (Gordon et al., 1993).

  4. 4.

    The introduction of this incongruent box information (for, e.g., a small box that is heavy or a large box that is light) will negatively impact the differentiation and accuracy of weight estimates. Thus, we expect the participants’ differentiations of weights to be better in response to the PLD condition that presents only the actor point-lights than to the condition that presents both the box and actor point-lights.

An interesting corollary outcome of our stimulus development is that the actor participates in a size–weight illusion protocol (Charpentier, 1891; Murray, Ellis, Bandomir, & Ross, 1999) that provides a unique opportunity to investigate how other nonkinematic and nonobject visual information impacts the accuracy of the observer estimates. The size–weight illusion refers to the phenomenon wherein an actor, when lifting objects of differing volumes and identical weight, perceives the smaller objects as being heavier. The illusion persists even when the actor is aware that the weights are identical, and does not lessen over the course of repeated lifting—even though the motor outputs associated with the lift cease to reflect the actor’s perception of differing weights (Ellis & Lederman, 1993; Flournoy, 1894; Nyssen & Bourdon, 1956). Under these lifting conditions, it is possible that comparisons of the weights estimated under the full-PLD conditions and the full-vision video condition will provide evidence of other information that discloses the actor’s differing perceptions of weight across box sizes of the same weight. That is, for example, the observer may estimate a weight to be heavier because of facial cues associated with the actor’s perception of the weight. This possibility is particularly noteworthy with respect to recent evidence that observer perceptions of lifting movements are more sensitive to information concerning actor effort than to lifted weight (Shim, Carlton, & Kim, 2004). If affective information related to the actor’s perception of the lifted weight impacts the observer perception of the lifted weight, then the observer should be less able to differentiate weights under the full-vision video condition.

Method

Stimulus generation

A naïve actor with considerable weight-lifting experience lifted a series of three boxes that were equal in width (39 cm) and height (18 cm) but that differed in depth (small = 18 cm, medium = 23 cm, large = 25 cm). All three boxes were mounted with a horizontal grasping bar and designed so that their weight could be manipulated through the addition or subtraction of weights. In the lightest condition, the boxes were manipulated such that they each weighed 25 lb. In the middle-weight condition, the boxes were manipulated to weigh 50 lb. In the heavy weight condition, the boxes were manipulated to weigh 75 lb. The actor performed three lifts of each size–weight box combination.

Prior to initiating the lifts, 29 reflective markers were affixed to the actor’s head, shoulders, elbows, wrists, hips, knees, and feet. The third of each of the actor’s lifts was recorded via a VICON motion capture system (VICON Motion Capture Nexus software; VICON Motion Systems, Lake Forest, CA), which captured the displacement of the reflective markers, and an HD camera (HV30 Camcorder, Canon, Japan), which captured video of the lifts in profile. For each lift, the actor bent at the knees, grasped the handle of the box, stood up straight to lift the box to hip level, and lowered it back to its original position (i.e., a deadlift). The actor performed each lift with his feet, hands, and gaze arranged according to markers affixed to the floor, grasping bar, and facing wall, respectively. He was also required to perform his movements in time with a metronome (45 beats/min), which was employed as a soft control against the actor adopting a variety of paces throughout the series of lifts. A chair was placed behind the actor in order to standardize the lift position for each box. The actor was asked to bend, grasp, and lift the box on consecutive beats of the metronome, but was not constrained by this rhythm, nor was he required to finish the lift on the fourth beat.

Although the metronome encouraged the actor to assume similar positions, purchases, postures, and general movement timings from one lift to the next, for the purposes of our manipulation, it was important that we establish kinematic differences between box lifts of different weight. Accordingly, we extracted and compared kinematic variables from the resultant PLDs that have been identified as influencing observers’ perceptions of lifted weight: dwell time, maximum hip angle, and lift velocity (see Shim & Carlton, 1997, for a review). Dwell time represents the period of time between the actor purchasing the box handle and the moment that the box is lifted off the ground. Using a custom code created in MATLAB, a mean dwell time for each box weight was computed in milliseconds. A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) did not reveal differences between the dwell times associated with different box weights, F(2, 8) = 1.089, p = .395. However, a review of the means revealed a systematic increase in the dwell period as the box weights increased (25 lb = 27.3 ms, 50 lb = 39.7 ms, 75 lb = 64.0 ms). Hip angle was defined as the angle between the leg and trunk segments and was calculated using the VICON coordinate data. Analysis of the hip angle at the peak box position revealed no differences between box weights, F(2, 8) = 2.553, p = .158, (25 lb = 129.2°, 50 lb = 129.7°, 75 lb = 127°). Lift velocity was determined via the angular velocity (deg/s) about the hip angle. An analysis of these data confirmed that as the box weight increased, the lift velocity decreased (25 lb = 0.72°/s, 50 lb = 0.62°/s, 75 lb = 0.42°/s), F(2, 8) = 7.34, p = .024.

Upon completing three lifts of a particular size–weight box, the actor was presented with another box size of the same weight. Once all three sizes had been lifted three times at a given weight, the actor was asked to declare his perception of the relative weights of the three box sizes. For each weight, the actor indicated that the small box was the heaviest and the large box was the lightest. The order of box size presentation was randomized within weights; however, the weights were presented in descending order, to minimize the impact of fatigue. The actor was not provided with any information regarding the weights of the boxes, nor was he permitted to see any weights being added or taken away from the boxes.

All grids and background reference frames were removed from the VICON recordings to create the PLD stimuli. The static-box and dynamic-box conditions reduced the display to only the four reflective markers that had been affixed to the corners of each box. In the static-box condition, the markers were presented at rest, and in the dynamic-box condition, the markers were presented in motion. In the actor condition, the display was degraded to only the point-light markers associated with the actor’s body. The actor-and-box condition presented all VICON markers against the black background. Finally, the full-video condition was created from the full-vision video captured by the HD camera. Each viewing condition was created from the same lift video for each specific size and weight condition, and all PLDs and videos were presented from the same profile perspective. Each viewing condition is presented in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1
figure 1

The viewing conditions for the (A) full-video, (B) actor-and-box, (C) actor, and (D) dynamic-box and static-box conditions

Participants

The participants were 13 individuals with normal or corrected-to-normal vision, recruited from the McMaster University community (eight males, five females; mean age = 23.1 years). All individuals provided informed consent according to the guidelines set out by the Declaration Helsinki (1954) and by the McMaster University Research Ethics Board prior to participating.

Procedure

Participants viewed the third of each of the actor’s lifts of each box in five viewing blocks (static-box, dynamic-box, actor, actor-and-box, and full-video) and provided weight estimates of the box lifted in each trial presentation. All participants viewed the static-box condition first and the full-video condition last. The dynamic-box, actor, and actor-and-box conditions were presented in a random order. All lifts were presented on a 15-in. AOC LM 760 liquid crystal display computer monitor with a 1,280 × 1,024 pixel resolution setting.

Each block consisted of 27 pseudorandomized trials. A trial consisted of a 3-s preparation period, followed by the presentation of the actual lift. Participants verbally indicated an estimation of the box weight in pounds to the experimenter. Prior to the first lift presentation in each block, and following every third lift presentation, the participant was presented an anchor presentation of the middle-weighted, medium-sized box. This anchor presentation included a caption indicating that the weight of the presented box was 50 lb. This manipulation was designed to assist the participants in calibrating their weight judgments. Each size–weight combination was presented three times in each block and did not appear in more than two consecutive trials. The trial order was balanced such that all nine size–weight combinations followed an anchor presentation at least once.

Dependent measures

Although it has been shown that the weight of lifted objects can be estimated accurately through PLD observation (Runeson & Frykholm, 1981, 1983), in this experiment we were more interested in the participants’ ability to differentiate objects of various weights. Accordingly, the absolute weight estimate for each trial was used as the primary dependent measure for all analyses, rather than an error value that represented the actual accuracy of the estimates.

Analysis

We conducted the following hypothesis-specific analyses:

  1. 1.

    Our first hypothesis was that participants would differentiate the lifted weights accurately when presented with arrays that included only the actor kinematic information. To test this, the weight estimates for the actor condition were compared in a 3 (weight: 25, 50, 75 lb) × 3 (size: small, middle, large) ANOVA.

  2. 2.

    Second, in order to test the hypothesis that the addition of kinematic information associated with the lift would improve the differentiation of weight estimates, we compared weight estimates in a 3 (viewing condition: static-box, dynamic-box, actor-and-box) × 3 (weight: 25, 50, 75 lb) × 3 (size: small, middle, large) ANOVA.

  3. 3.

    We tested the hypothesis that weight estimates would reflect object size when the presented array contained only box size information through a 3 (weight: 25, 50, 75 lb) × 3 (size: small, middle, or large) ANOVA of the weight estimates provided in the static-box condition.

  4. 4.

    Our fourth hypothesis was that this incongruent box information would negatively impact the differentiation of weights estimated on the bases of actor kinematic information. Accordingly, we analyzed the weight estimates in a 2 (viewing condition: actor, actor-and-box) × 3 weight (25, 50, 75 lb) × 3 size (small, middle, large) ANOVA.

We also performed a pair of analyses in order to investigate how additional, nonkinematic and nonbox, information would impact the weight estimates:

  1. 5.

    Weight estimates were compared in a 2 (viewing condition: actor-and-box, full-video) × 3 (weight: 25, 50, 75 lb) × 3 (size: small, middle, large) ANOVA.

  2. 6.

    Furthermore, we employed Pearson’s correlation methodology to determine the relationship between the absolute weight estimate and the true object weight for each viewing condition. The resultant r 2 values reflected the amount of variation in weight perception that was accounted for by the visual information available each condition. These r 2 values were then subjected to a five-viewing-condition (static-box, dynamic-box, actor, actor-and-box, full-video) one-way ANOVA.

Tukey’s HSD post hoc methodology was used to decompose all significant effects (p < .05) involving more than two means.

Results

The results of the first analysis did not yield any significant weight estimation differences in the actor condition (grand mean = 53 ± 11.3 lb). Interestingly, this suggests that the participants were unable to differentiate the weights of lifted objects solely on the bases of kinematic lift information.

The second analysis revealed a significant Viewing Condition × Weight interaction, F(4, 48) = 3.24, p < .019. Post hoc analysis of this interaction revealed that participants differentiated the 25- and 75-lb boxes in the actor-and-box condition, but not in the dynamic-box or static-box conditions (Fig. 2), indicating that lift velocity information independently was not sufficient kinematic information for the generation of appropriate estimates. This analysis also yielded significant Size × Weight, F(4, 48) = 5.91, p < .001, and Size × Viewing Condition, F(4, 48) = 34.07, p < .001, interactions.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Participant mean (± SE) weight estimates (in pounds), plotted as a function of box weight and PLD viewing conditions of increasing kinematic content

The third analysis revealed a significant main effect of box size, F(2, 16) = 148.8, p < .001, in which participants estimated the large box to be heavier than the middle box, which in turn was estimated to be heavier than the small box (Fig. 3). This finding supports the hypothesis that weight estimates reflect box size when the visual presentation does not include any kinematic information.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Participant mean (± SE) weight estimates (in pounds), plotted as a function of box size in the static box-viewing condition

The comparison of actor-condition estimates with actor-and-box estimates did not yield a significant Viewing Condition × Weight interaction (p = .10), suggesting that box size information does not confound the perception of lifted weight. However, the analysis did yield a significant weight main effect, F(2, 24) = 5.83, p < .01, post hoc analysis of which revealed that the heavy box (55.2 ± 3.3 lb) was estimated to be significantly heavier than the light box (48.7 ± 2.9 lb), regardless of the presence of box size information. This finding is particularly relevant, insofar as it offers support for Hypothesis 1, that appropriate perceptions of weight may be gleaned from kinematic lift information.

The comparison of the full-vision video estimates with the actor-and-box PLD estimates revealed significant Viewing Condition × Size, F(2, 24) = 6.27, p < .01, and Size × Weight, F(4, 48) = 11.03, p < .001, interactions. Post hoc decomposition of the Viewing Condition × Size interaction revealed that in the video condition, participants estimated the weight of the small box (45.1 ± 3.0 lb) to be less than the weight of the medium (55.8 ± 4.9 lb) and large (57.4 ± 4.3 lb) boxes, but that no differences emerged between these estimates and any of those associated with the actor-and-box viewing condition (small, 53.0 ± 2.9 lb; middle, 48.7 ± 3.5 lb; large, 53.8 ± 3.7 lb). Post hoc analysis of the Size × Weight interaction revealed no differences between the participants’ weight estimates associated with box sizes in the 25-lb condition (small, 42.2 ± 3.0 lb; middle, 42.3 ± 2.5 lb; large, 45.6 ± 3.1 lb). However, in the 50-lb condition, participants estimated the large box (59.9 ± 2.8 lb) to be heavier than the small (46.7 ± 2.9 lb) and middle-sized (48.9 ± 1.9 lb) boxes, and in the 75-lb condition, participants estimated the middle-sized box (65.4 ± 5.2 lb) to be heavier than the small box (58.3 ± 2.9 lb), with the large-box (61.3 ± 4.6) estimates being intermediate, but not significantly different from either.

Of greatest interest for this analysis, however, was a significant Viewing Condition × Weight interaction, F(2, 24) = 9.35, p < .001. Post hoc decomposition of this analysis indicated that in the video condition, participants were able to differentiate each of the three weights; however, when presented with the PLD information, the participants could only differentiate the heaviest box from the lightest (Fig. 4).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Participant mean (± SE) weight estimates (in pounds), plotted as a function box weight under the full-video and actor-and-box conditions

The analysis of the r 2 values obtained from the Pearson correlations between the absolute weight estimates and the true object weights revealed a significant main effect of viewing condition, F(4, 48) = 20.53, p < .001. The post hoc decomposition of this effect revealed that the visual information associated with the full-video condition accounted for the greatest amount of accuracy in weight estimations, and that no other intermediate differences emerged (Fig. 5).

Fig. 5
figure 5

A plot of the correlations (r 2) between the observers’ mean weight estimates and the object’s actual weight across viewing conditions

Discussion

In this study, the visual information that was available to observers in PLD presentations of an actor lifting boxes of various sizes and weights was manipulated. This was done in order to determine the relative degrees to which kinematic movement information and extrinsic box size information contribute and covary in order to influence observers’ perceptual estimates of lifted box weights. In this regard, a review of our results indicated that kinematic information is a powerful determinant in the generation of movement perceptions that can differentiate the weights of manipulated objects. This finding is in agreement with the results generated by the Alaerts group (Alaerts, Senot, et al., 2010; see also Buckingham, Wong, Tang, Gribble, & Goodale, 2013; Reichelt, Ash, Baugh, Johansson, & Flanagan, 2013). However, in the present work, neither kinematic information about the velocity of the lifted box nor the angular displacements and velocities of the actor’s joints were independently sufficient for observers to make appropriate estimates. It would appear that such perceptions depend on an integration of both the actor’s and object’s kinematics. This finding is particularly interesting when one considers the incongruent way in which box size information serves to influence observed weight perceptions in the absence of kinematic information (i.e., the static-box condition). Overall, it appears that visually available kinematics can work to offset perceptions of object weight based on size information, but that this mechanism requires a minimum amount of kinematic information. The present study suggests that this minimum is in excess of information concerning the object in motion.

The design of this study also permitted the opportunity to investigate the influence that additional nonkinematic and nonobject information has on observer perceptions of lifted weight. In this regard, Shim et al. (2004) demonstrated that extraneous visual information concerning the effort that an actor exerts influences his or her observers’ weight perceptions. Because our stimulus generation protocol required our actor to lift boxes under conditions of the size–weight illusion (Charpentier, 1891; Murray et al., 1999), we were able to test the hypothesis that observers’ perceptions of actor effort impact weight estimations in a manner that offsets the influences associated with actor kinematic or box-size information. Specifically, in each weight category, the actor perceived the smaller box as being the heaviest, and the larger box as being the lightest, and assuming that the actor’s effort reflected his perceptions, we reasoned that the full-vision video would be associated with confounded weight estimates. However, this was clearly not the case. Rather, the additional visual information associated with the video presentation actually led to observer estimates that were better differentiated than those under the PLD conditions. Thus, visual information beyond kinematics and object characteristics contributes to accurate dynamic perceptions. The nature of this information, however, remains uncertain. With regard to how much this information relates to visual aspects of an actor’s effort, we must remain aware of the way that force outputs are mediated in size–weight illusion contexts. An actor’s expectations about an object’s weight can be observed in the initial load phase of the lift. If the predictions about the weight of the box are mistaken, then the lift’s dwell time will be inappropriate, and lift-off will occur either sooner or later than expected, if at all. In either case, this inappropriate force output leads to reflex mediated changes in force loading (Gordon et al., 1991; R. S. Johansson & Westling, 1988; Westling & Johansson, 1984), and over the course of a few lifts, the forces scale to the actual weight of the lifted box, although the illusion still persists (Flanagan & Beltzner, 2000). Thus, we must interpret our data as evidence that any visual information regarding the actor’s true effort is more salient to the observer than are visual cues regarding the actor’s perception of effort.