Abstract
The tendency for observers to overestimate slant is not simply a visual illusion but can also occur with another sense, such as proprioception, as in the case of overestimation of self-body tilt. In the present study, distortion in the perception of body tilt was examined as a function of gender and multisensory spatial information. We used a full-body-tilt apparatus to test when participants experienced being tilted by 45°, with visual and auditory cues present or absent. Body tilt was overestimated in all conditions, with the largest bias occurring when there were no visual or auditory cues. Both visual and auditory information independently improved performance. We also found a gender difference, with women exhibiting more bias in the absence of auditory information and more improvement when auditory information was added. The findings support the view that perception of body tilt is multisensory and that women more strongly utilize auditory information in such multisensory spatial judgments.
Article PDF
References
Afonso, S. (2001). Landscape and environment in architecture and urban design. In F. O. R. Pereira, R. Ruther, R. V. G. Souza, S. Afonso, & J. A. B. de Cunha Neto (Eds.), Proceedings of the Eighteenth International Conference on Passive and Low Energy Architecture (pp. 501-506). Florianópolis, Brazil: Organizing Committee of the 18th International PLEA conference. Available at www.arq.ufsc.br/~soniaa/sonia/paperpleacomimagens.pdf.
Aksamit, G., & Husak, W. (1983). Feedback influences on the skill of putting. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 56, 19–22.
Allen, J. G., & Haccoun, D. M. (1976). Sex differences in emotionality: A multidimensional approach. Human Relations, 29, 711–722.
American Automobile Association (1996). California/Nevada tour book. Heathrow, FL: Author.
Arthur, E. J., Hancock, P. A., & Chrysler, S. T. (1997). The perception of spatial layout in real and virtual worlds. Ergonomics, 40, 69–77.
Ashmead, D. H., Davis, D. L., & Northington, A. (1995). Contribution of listeners’ approaching motion to auditory distance perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 21, 239–256.
Bertamini, M., Yang, T. L., & Proffitt, D. R. (1998). Relative size perception at a distance is best at eye level. Perception & Psychophysics, 6, 673–682.
Blier, M. J., & Blier-Wilson, L. A. (1989). Gender differences in selfrated emotional expressiveness. Sex Roles, 21, 287–295.
Bringoux, L., Nougier, V., Barraud, P. A., Marin, L., & Raphel, C. (2003). Contribution of somesthetic information to the perception of body orientation in the pitch dimension. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 909–923.
Bronkhorst, A. W., & Houtgast, T. (1999). Auditory distance perception in rooms. Nature, 397, 517–520.
Bronstein, A. (1999). The interaction of otolith and proprioceptive information in the perception of verticality: The effects of labyrinthine and CNS disease. In B. Cohen & B. J. M. Hess (Eds.), Otolith function in spatial orientation and movement (Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 871, pp. 324–333). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.
Brungart, D. S., & Rabinowitz, W. M. (1999). Auditory localization of nearby sources: Head-related transfer functions. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 106, 1465–1479.
Cahill, L. (2005). His brain, her brain. Scientific American, 292, 40–47.
Casey, M. B. (1996). Understanding individual differences in spatial ability within females: A nature/nurture interactionist framework. Developmental Review, 16, 241–260.
Casey, M. B., & Brabeck, M. M. (1989). Exceptions to the male advantage on a spatial task: Family handedness and college major as factors identifying women who excel. Neuropsychologia, 27, 689–696.
Dalla Pozza, R., Kleinmann, A., Kozlik-Feldmann, R., & Netz, H. (2006). Tilt table testing in childhood: improved sensitivity by noninvasive haemodynamic monitoring devices? Acta Cardiologica, 61, 615–621.
Day, B. L., & Cole, J. (2002). Vestibular-evoked postural responses in the absence of somatosensory information. Brain, 125, 2081–2088.
Della Casa, V., Höfer, I., Weiner, I., & Feldon, J. (1998). The effects of smoking on acoustic prepulse inhibition in healthy men and women. Psychopharmacology, 137, 362–368.
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Higashiyama, A., & Koga, K. (1998). Apparent body tilt and postural aftereffect. Perception & Psychophysics, 60, 331–347.
Isableu, B., & Vuillerme, N. (2006). Differential integration of kinaesthetic signals to postural control. Experimental Brain Research, 174, 763–768.
Ito, Y., & Gresty, M. A. (1997). Subjective postural orientation and visual vertical during slow pitch tilt for the seated human subject. Aviation, Space, & Environmental Medicine, 68, 3–12.
Jeka, J. J., Easton, R. D., Bentzen, B. L., & Lackner, J. R. (1996). Haptic cues for orientation and postural control in sighted and blind individuals. Perception & Psychophysics, 58, 409–423.
Jewell, J. G. (1998). The misperception of body tilt: Support for an ecologically-guided multisensory representation of space. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Kent State University.
Jewell, J. G., & McBeath, M. K. (1998). A mapping of the distortion in the representation of proprioceptive space. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 39, S1096.
King, A. J., Schnupp, J. W. H., & Doubell, T. P. (2001). The shape of ears to come: dynamic coding of auditory space. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 261–270.
Kinsella-Shaw, J. M., Shaw, B., & Turvey, M. T. (1992). Perceiving “walk-on-able” slopes. Ecological Psychology, 4, 223–239.
Lawton, C. A. (1994). Gender differences in way-finding strategies: Relationship to spatial ability and spatial anxiety. Sex Roles, 30, 765–779.
Lawton, C. A. (1996). Strategies for indoor way finding: The role of orientation. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 16, 137–145.
Lawton, C. A., & Morrin, K. A. (1999). Gender differences in pointing accuracy in computer-simulated 3D mazes. Sex Roles, 40, 73–92.
Levy, J. (1976). Cerebral lateralization and spatial ability. Behavior Genetics, 6, 171–188.
Linn, M. C., & Petersen, A. C. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 56, 1479–1498.
Little, A. D., Mershon, D. H., & Cox, P. H. (1992). Spectral content as a cue to perceived auditory distance. Perception, 21, 405–416.
Loomis, J. M., Klatzky, R. L., Philbeck, J. W., & Golledge, R. G. (1998). Assessing auditory distance perception using perceptually directed action. Perception & Psychophysics, 60, 966–980.
Manchester, D., Woollacott, M., Zederbauer-Hylton, N., & Marin, O. (1989). Visual, vestibular and somatosensory contributions to balance control in the older adult. Journal of Gerontology, 44, M118-M127.
McFadden, S. L., Henselman, L. W., & Zheng, X. Y. (1999). Sex differences in auditory sensitivity of chinchillas before and after exposure to impulse noise. Ear & Hearing, 20, 164–174.
Meeuwsen, H. J. (1991). Variables affecting perceptual boundaries in bipedal stair climbing. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 72, 539–543.
Mershon, D. H., Ballenger, W. L., Little, A. D., McMurtry, P. L., & Buchanan, J. L. (1989). Effects of room reflectance and background noise on perceived auditory distance. Perception, 18, 403–416.
Miller, L. K., & Santoni, V. (1986). Sex differences in spatial abilities: Strategic and experiential correlates. Acta Psychologica, 62, 225–235.
Mittelstaedt, H. (1996). Somatic graviception. Biological Psychology, 42, 53–74.
Ooi, T. L., Wu, B., & He, Z. J. (2001). Distance determined by the angular declination below the horizon, Nature, 414, 197–200.
Parameswaran, G. (1995). Gender difference in horizontality performance before and after training. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 156, 105–113.
Perrone, J. A. (1982). Visual slant underestimation: A general model. Perception, 11, 641–654.
Proffitt, D. R., Bhalla, M., Gossweiler, R., & Midgett, J. (1995). Perceiving geographical slant. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 409–428.
Proffitt, D. R., Creem, S. H., & Zosh, W. D. (2001). Seeing mountains in molehills: Geographical-slant perception. Psychological Science, 12, 418–423.
Ries, M., Kim, H. J., Zalewski, C. K., Mastroianni, M. A., Moore, D. F., Brady, R. O., et al. (2007). Neuropathic and cerebrovascular correlates of hearing loss in Fabry disease. Brain, 130, 143–150.
Ross, H. E. (1974). Behaviour and perception in strange environments. London: Allen & Unwin.
Rossier, J., Haeberli, C., & Schenk, F. (2000). Auditory cues support place navigation in rats when associated with a visual cue. Behavioural Brain Research, 117, 209–214.
Seurinck, R., Vingerhoets, G., de Lange, F. P., & Achten, E. (2004). Does egocentric mental rotation elicit sex differences? NeuroImage, 23, 1440–1449.
Sholl, M. J. (1989). The relation between horizontality and rod-andframe and vestibular navigational performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 15, 110–125.
Stefanucci, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., Banton, T., & Epstein, W. (2005). Distances appear different on hills. Perception & Psychophysics, 67, 1052–1060.
Swerdlow, N. R., Auerbach, P., Monroe, S. M., Hartston, H., Geyer, M. A., & Braff, D. L. (1993). Men are more inhibited than women by weak prepulses. Biological Psychiatry, 34, 253–260.
Swerdlow, N. R., Filion, D., Geyer, M. A., & Braff, D. L. (1995). “Normal” personality correlates of sensorimotor, cognitive, and visuospatial gating. Biological Psychiatry, 37, 286–299.
Tran, T. V., Letowski, T., & Abouchacra, K. S. (2000). Evaluation of acoustic beacon characteristics for navigation tasks. Ergonomics, 43, 807–827.
Tremblay, L., & Elliott, D. (2007). Sex differences in judging selforientation: the morphological horizon and body pitch. BMC Neuroscience, 8, 1–8.
Trousselard, M., Barraud, P.-A., Nougier, V., Raphel, C., & Cian, C. (2004). Contribution of tactile and interoceptive cues to the perception of the direction of gravity. Cognitive Brain Research, 20, 355–362.
Ward, S., Newcombe, N., & Overton, W. (1986). Turn left at the church, or three miles north: A study of direction giving and sex differences. Environment & Behavior, 18, 192–213.
Wightman, F., & Kistler, D. (2005). Measurement and validation of human HRTFs for use in hearing research. Acta Acustica United With Acustica, 91, 429–439.
Witelson, S. F., Beresh, H., & Kigar, D. L. (2006). Intelligence and brain size in 100 postmortem brains: sex, lateralization and age factors. Brain, 129, 386–398.
Wraga, M., Duncan, L., Jacobs, E. C., Helt, M., & Church, J. (2006). Stereotype susceptibility narrows the gender gap in imagined self-rotation performance. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 813–819.
Zuidhoek, S., Kappers, A. M. L., & Postma, A. (2007). Haptic orientation perception: Sex differences and lateralization of functions. Neuropsychologia, 45, 332–341.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
This work was funded in part by NSF Grant BCS-0318313.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Naylor, Y.K., McBeath, M.K. Gender differences in spatial perception of body tilt. Perception & Psychophysics 70, 199–207 (2008). https://doi.org/10.3758/PP.70.2.199
Received:
Accepted:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/PP.70.2.199