Abstract
Can directed actions unconsciously influence higher order cognitive processing? We investigated how movement interventions affected participants’ ability to solve a classic insight problem. The participants attempted to solve Maier’s two-string problem while occasionally taking exercise breaks during which they moved their arms either in a manner related to the problem’s solution (swing group) or in a manner inconsistent with the solution (stretch group). Although most of the participants were unaware of the relationship between their arm movement exercises and the problem-solving task, the participants who moved their arms in a manner that suggested the problem’s solution were more likely to solve the problem than were those who moved their arms in other ways. Consistent with embodied theories of cognition, these findings show that actions influence thought and, furthermore, that we can implicitly guide people toward insight by directing their actions.
References
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). New York: Academic Press.
Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 22, 577–660. doi:10.1017/S0140525X99002149
Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639
Beilock, S. L., & Holt, L. E. (2007). Embodied preference judgments: Can likeability be driven by the motor system? Psychological Science, 18, 51–57. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01848.x
Cleves, M. A., Gould, W. W., & Gutierrez, R. G. (2002). An introduction to survival analysis using Stata. College Station, TX: Stata Press.
Cook, S. W., Mitchell, Z., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2008). Gesturing makes learning last. Cognition, 106, 1047–1058. doi:10.1016/ j.cognition.2007.04.010
Driver, J., & Spence, C. (2004). Crossmodal spatial attention: Evidence from human performance. In C. Spence & J. Driver (Eds.), Crossmodal space and crossmodal attention (pp. 179–220). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1980). Analogical problem solving. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 306–355. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(80)90013-4
Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Schema induction and analogical transfer. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 1–38. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(83)90002-6
Glenberg, A. M. (1997). What memory is for. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 20, 1–55. doi:10.1017/S0140525X97000010
Glenberg, A. M., Sato, M., & Cattaneo, L. (2008). Use-induced motor plasticity affects the processing of abstract and concrete language. Current Biology, 18, R290-R291. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.02.036
Hegarty, M. (2004). Mechanical reasoning as mental simulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 280–285. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.04.001
Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 24, 849–878.
Klauer, K. C., & Zhao, Z. (2004). Double dissociations in visual and spatial short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 355–381. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.355
Landrum, R. E. (1990). Maier’s (1931) two-string problem revisited: Evidence for spontaneous transfer. Psychological Reports, 67, 1079–1088. doi:10.2466/PR0.67.8.1079-1088
Logie, R. H. (1995). Visuo-spatial working memory. Hove, U.K.: Erlbaum.
Macaluso, E., Frith, C. D., & Driver, J. (2000). Modulation of human visual cortex by crossmodal spatial attention. Science, 289, 1206–1208. doi:10.1126/science.289.5482.1206
Maier, N. R. F. (1931). Reasoning in humans: II. The solution of a problem and its appearance in consciousness. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 12, 181–194. doi:10.1037/h0071361
Martin, A. (2007). The representation of object concepts in the brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 25–45. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190143
Metcalfe, J., & Wiebe, D. (1987). Intuition in insight and noninsight problem solving. Memory & Cognition, 15, 238–246.
Reed, S. K., Ernst, G. W., & Banerji, R. (1974). The role of analogy in transfer between similar problem states. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 436–450. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(74)90020-6
Rubin, D. C. (2006). The basic-systems model of episodic memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 277–311.
Smith, S. M., & Blankenship, S. E. (1991). Incubation and the persistence of fixation in problem solving. American Journal of Psychology, 104, 61–87. doi:10.2307/1422851
Stepper, S., & Strack, F. (1993). Proprioceptive determinants of emotional and nonemotional feelings. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 64, 211–220. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.64.2.211
Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 54, 768–777. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.5.768
Thomas, L. E., & Lleras, A. (2007). Moving eyes and moving thought: On the spatial compatibility between eye movements and cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 663–668.
Thomas, L. E., & Lleras, A. (2009). Covert shifts of attention function as an implicit aid to insight. Cognition, 111, 168–174. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.01.005
Weisberg, R. W., & Alba, J. W. (1981). An examination of the alleged role of “fixation” in the solution of several “insight” problems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 169–192. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.110.2.169
Wells, G. L., & Petty, R. E. (1980). The effects of overt head movements on persuasion: Compatibility and incompatibility of responses. Basic & Applied Social Psychology, 1, 219–230. doi:10.1207/ s15324834basp0103_2
Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 625–636.
Zwaan, R. A. (1999). Embodied cognition, perceptual symbols, and situation models. Discourse Processes, 28, 81–88.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Thomas, L.E., Lleras, A. Swinging into thought: Directed movement guides insight in problem solving. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 16, 719–723 (2009). https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.16.4.719
Received:
Accepted:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.16.4.719
Keywords
- Spatial Compatibility
- Insight Problem
- String Problem
- Posttest Questionnaire
- Stretch Group