Advertisement

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

, Volume 15, Issue 6, pp 1166–1173 | Cite as

The impact of discredited evidence

  • David A. LagnadoEmail author
  • Nigel Harvey
Brief Reports

Abstract

How do people revise their beliefs when evidence is discredited? In three studies, mock jurors read simplified criminal cases and then judged the probability that a suspect was guilty on the basis of sequentially presented evidence. Study 1 showed an extension effect: When two items of incriminating evidence were presented, a subsequent discrediting of the second item also lessened belief in the first item, irrespective of whether it was directly related to the discredited item. Study 2 showed that this effect depended on the order of evidence presentation: When the discrediting evidence was presented early, rather than late, in the sequence, there was no extension to unrelated items. Study 3 showed that the extension effect held only when items of evidence were both incriminating or both exonerating, but not when they were mixed. To explain these findings, we draw on coherence-based models of juror reasoning and propose that people group evidence according to its direction with respect to the guilt hypothesis.

Keywords

Early Condition Crime Scene Extension Effect Mock Juror Similar Neighbor 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Carlson, K. A., & Russo, J. E. (2001). Biased interpretation of evidence by mock jurors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7, 91–103.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Hastie, R. (Ed.) (1993). Inside the juror: The psychology of juror decision Making. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Hatvany, N., & Strack, F. (1980). The impact of a discredited key witness. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 10, 490–509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Heider, F. (1946). Attitudes and cognitive organization. Journal of Psychology, 21, 107–112.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Hogarth, R. M., & Einhorn, H. J. (1992). Order effects in belief updating: The belief-adjustment model. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 1–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Kennedy, T. D., & Haygood, R. C. (1992). The discrediting effect in eyewitness testimony. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22, 70–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Pearl, J. (1988). Probabilistic reasoning in intelligent systems: Networks of plausible inference (rev. 2nd print.). San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.Google Scholar
  8. Pennington, N., & Hastie, R. (1992). Explaining the evidence: Tests of the story model for juror decision making. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 62, 189–206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Schum, D. A., & Martin, A. W. (1982). Formal and empirical research on cascaded inference in jurisprudence. Law & Society Review, 17, 105–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Simon, D., & Holyoak, K. J. (2002). Structural dynamics of cognition: From consistency theories to constraint satisfaction. Personality & Social Psychology Review, 6, 283–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Thagard, P. (2000). Coherence in thought and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Cognitive, Perceptual and Brain SciencesUniversity College LondonLondonEngland

Personalised recommendations