Abstract
Although memory researchers know about primacy and recency effects, it is unclear whether students are aware of these effects and incorporate them when making judgments of learning (JOLs). The present research examined how participants use serial position information (extrinsic cues) when making JOLs after studying each item and showed that participants rely on the intrinsic qualities of the items and underestimate primacy and recency effects. However, when participants made JOLs prior to studying each item and engaged in multiple study-test sessions, their JOLs accurately reflected recall, as well as when serial position information was explicitly provided during the study phase. The findings are interpreted in a cue utilization framework and suggest that under certain conditions, participants can predict primacy and recency effects.
Article PDF
Similar content being viewed by others
References
Arbuckle, T. Y., & Cuddy, L. L. (1969). Discrimination of item strength at time of presentation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 126–131.
Begg, I., Duft, S., Lalonde, P., Melnick, R., & Sanvito, J. (1989). Memory predictions are based on ease of processing. Journal of Memory & Language, 28, 610–632.
Benjamin, A. S. (2003). Predicting and postdicting the effects of word frequency on memory. Memory & Cognition, 31, 297–305.
Benjamin, A. S., Bjork, R. A., & Schwartz, B. L. (1998). The mismeasure of memory: When retrieval fluency is misleading as a metacognitive index. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 55–68.
Brooks, B. M. (1999). Primacy and recency in primed free association and associative cued recall. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, 479–485.
Castel, A. D. (2007). The adaptive and strategic use of memory by older adults: Evaluative processing and value-directed remembering. In A. S. Benjamin & B. H. Ross (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 48, pp. 225–270). San Diego: Academic Press.
Castel, A. D., Benjamin, A. S., Craik, F. I. M., & Watkins, M. J. (2002). The effects of aging on selectivity and control in short-term recall. Memory & Cognition, 30, 1078–1085.
Castel, A. D., Farb, N. A. S., & Craik, F. I. M. (2007). Memory for general and specific value information in younger and older adults: Measuring the limits of strategic control. Memory & Cognition, 35, 689–700.
Castel, A. D., & Logan, J. M. (2007, November). Learning about the spacing effect: The role of experience and feedback. Poster presented at the 48th Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Long Beach, CA.
Castel, A. D., McCabe, D. P., & Roediger, H. L., III (2007). Illusions of competence and overestimation of associative memory for identical items: Evidence from judgments of learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 107–111.
Crowder, R. G. (1969). Behavioral strategies in immediate memory. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 8, 524–528.
deWinstanley, P. A., & Bjork, E. L. (2004). Processing strategies and the generation effect: Implications for making a better reader. Memory & Cognition, 32, 945–955.
Dunlosky, J., & Hertzog, C. (2000). Updating knowledge about encoding strategies: A componential analysis of learning about strategy effectiveness from task experience. Psychology & Aging, 15, 462–474.
Dunlosky, J., & Matvey, G. (2001). Empirical analysis of the intrinsic-extrinsic distinction of judgments of learning (JOLs): Effects of relatedness and serial position on JOLs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 27, 1180–1191.
Dunlosky, J., & Nelson, T. O. (1994). Does the sensitivity of judgments of learning (JOLs) to the effects of various study activities depend on when the JOLs occur? Journal of Memory & Language, 33, 545–565.
Dunlosky, J., & Thiede, K. W. (1998). What makes people study more? An evaluation of factors that affect people’s self-paced study and yield “labor-and-gain” effects. Acta Psychologica, 98, 37–56.
Finn, B., & Metcalfe, J. (2007). The role of memory for past test in the underconfidence with practice effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 33, 238–244.
Ghodsian, D., Bjork, R. A., & Benjamin, A. S. (1997). Evaluating training during training: Obstacles and opportunities. In M. A. Quiñones & A. Ehrenstein (Eds.), Training for a rapidly changing workplace: Applications of psychological research (pp. 63–88). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Glanzer, M., & Cunitz, A. R. (1966). Two storage mechanisms in free recall. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 5, 351–360.
Guttentag, R., & Carroll, D. (1998). Memorability judgments for high- and low-frequency words. Memory & Cognition, 26, 951–958.
Hertzog, C., Dunlosky, J., Robinson, E., & Kidder, D. (2003). Encoding fluency is a cue used for judgments about learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 29, 22–34.
Kelley, C. M., & Jacoby, L. L. (1996). Adult geocentricism: Subjective experience versus analytical bases for judgment. Journal of Memory & Language, 35, 157–175.
Kimball, D. R., & Metcalfe, J. (2003). Delaying judgments of learning affects memory, not metamemory. Memory & Cognition, 31, 918–929.
King, J. F., Zechmeister, E. B., & Shaughnessy, J. J. (1980). Judgments of knowing: The influence of retrieval practice. American Journal of Psychology, 93, 329–343.
Koriat, A. (1997). Monitoring one’s own knowledge during study: A cue-utilization approach to judgments of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126, 349–370.
Koriat, A., & Bjork, R. A. (2005). Illusions of competence in monitoring one’s knowledge during study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 31, 187–194.
Koriat, A., & Bjork, R. A. (2006). Illusions of competence during study can be remedied by manipulations that enhance learners’ sensitivity to retrieval conditions at test. Memory & Cognition, 34, 959–972.
Koriat, A., Bjork, R. A., Sheffer, L., & Bar, S. (2004). Predicting one’s own forgetting: The role of experience-based and theory-based processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 643–656.
Koriat, A., & Ma’ayan, H. (2005). The effects of encoding fluency and retrieval fluency on judgments of learning. Journal of Memory & Language, 52, 478–492.
Kornell, N., & Metcalfe, J. (2006). Study efficacy and the region of proximal learning framework. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 32, 609–622.
Matvey, G., Dunlosky, J., & Guttentag, R. (2001). Fluency of retrieval at study affects judgments of learning (JOLs): An analytic or nonanalytic basis for JOLs? Memory & Cognition, 29, 222–233.
Matvey, G., Dunlosky, J., & Schwartz, B. (2006). The effects of categorical relatedness on judgments of learning (JOLs). Memory, 14, 253–261.
Murdock, B. B. (1962). The serial position effect of free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 482–488.
Nelson, T. O. (1993). Judgments of learning and the allocation of study time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 269–273.
Nelson, T. O., & Dunlosky, J. (1991). When people’s judgments of learning are extremely accurate at predicting subsequent recall: The “delayed JOL effect.” Psychological Science, 2, 267–270.
Nelson, T. O., Dunlosky, J., Graf, A., & Narens, L. (1994). Utilization of metacognitive judgments in the allocation of study during multitrial learning. Psychological Science, 5, 207–213.
Nelson, T. O., Narens, L., & Dunlosky, J. (2004). A revised methodology for research on metamemory: Pre-judgment recall and monitoring (PRAM). Psychological Methods, 9, 53–69.
Roediger, H. L., III, Gallo, D. A., & Dolan, P. O. (2001, November). Francis E. Nipher: The first memory researcher. Paper presented at the 42nd Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Orlando, FL.
Stigler, S. M. (1978). Some forgotten work on memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory, 4, 1–4.
Thorndike, E. L., & Lorge, I. (1944). The teacher’s word book of 30,000 words. New York: Columbia University, Teachers College Press.
Watkins, M. J., & Watkins, O. C. (1974). Processing of recency items for free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102, 488–493.
Waugh, N. C., & Norman, D. A. (1965). Primary memory. Psychological Review, 72, 89–104.
Welch, G. B., & Burnett, C. T. (1924). Is primacy a factor in association formation? American Journal of Psychology, 35, 396–401.
Zechmeister, E. B., & Shaughnessy, J. J. (1980). When you know that you know and when you think that you know but you don’t. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 15, 41–44.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
Many people provided valuable suggestions and insight regarding this research at various points; specifically,
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Castel, A.D. Metacognition and learning about primacy and recency effects in free recall: The utilization of intrinsic and extrinsic cues when making judgments of learning. Memory & Cognition 36, 429–437 (2008). https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.36.2.429
Received:
Accepted:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.36.2.429