Learning & Behavior

, Volume 38, Issue 3, pp 195–205 | Cite as

Opportunities and constraints when studying social learning: Developmental approaches and social factors

Articles

Abstract

Identifying social learning in wild populations is complicated by the relative lack of ability to conduct controlled experiments in natural habitats. Even in more controlled captive settings, tracking the innovation and spread of behavior among known individuals can be challenging, and these studies often suffer from a lack of ecological validity. In recent years, a host of new approaches have been undertaken to attempt to provide more quantitative control and empirical demonstration of social learning, both in the wild and in captive settings that more closely mimic natural contexts. Developmental approaches are being undertaken more regularly that allow us to study the ontogenetic trajectory of complex skills in a variety of taxa. Likewise, a spirited focus on the social context of social learning has emerged, and researchers have begun to meticulously analyze the influences of social systems and the characteristics of demonstrators and observers. Here, we provide a review of these studies and summarize the opportunities and constraints that exist when one attempts to study learning in social species. We suggest that although the study of social learning in nonhuman animals is becoming much more complex, addressing this complexity provides a fruitful model for understanding the evolution of human cultural behavior.

References

  1. Agostini, I., & Visalberghi, E. (2005). Social influences on the acquisition of sex-typical foraging patterns by juveniles in a group of wild tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus nigritus). American Journal of Primatology, 65, 335–351.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Amlacher, J., & Dugatkin, L. A. (2005). Preference for older over younger models during mate-choice copying in young guppies. Ethology Ecology & Evolution, 17, 161–169.Google Scholar
  3. Benskin, C. M. H., Mann, N. I., Lachlan, R. F., & Slater, P. J. B. (2002). Social learning directs feeding preferences in the zebra finch, Taeniopygia guttata. Animal Behaviour, 64, 823–828.Google Scholar
  4. Bentley, R. A., Hahn, M. W., & Shennan, S. J. (2004). Random drift and culture change. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 271, 1443–1450.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Biro, D., Inoue-Nakamura, N., Tonooka, R., Yamakoshi, G., Sousa, C., & Matsuzawa, T. (2003). Cultural innovation and transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees: Evidence from field experiments. Animal Cognition, 6, 213–223.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Bonnie, K. E., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2006). Affiliation promotes the transmission of a social custom: Handclasp grooming among captive chimpanzees. Primates, 47, 27–34.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Bonnie, K. E., Horner, V., Whiten, A., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2007). Spread of arbitrary conventions among chimpanzees: A controlled experiment. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 274, 367–372.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Boogert, N. J., Reader, S. M., Hoppitt, W., & Laland, K. N. (2008). The origin and spread of innovations in starlings. Animal Behaviour, 75, 1509–1518. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.09.033Google Scholar
  9. Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1988). An evolutionary model of social learning. In T. R. Zentall & B. G. Galef, Jr. (Eds.), Social learning: Psychological and biological perspectives (pp. 29–48). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  10. Breck, S. W., Williams, C. L., Beckmann, J. P., Matthews, S. M., Lackey, C. W., & Beecham, J. J. (2008). Using genetic relatedness to investigate the development of conflict behavior in black bears. Journal of Mammalogy, 89, 428–434.Google Scholar
  11. Brown, C., & Laland, K. N. (2003). Social learning in fishes: A review. Fish & Fisheries, 4, 280–288.Google Scholar
  12. Burkart, J. M., Fehr, E., Efferson, C., & van Schaik, C. (2007). Other-regarding preferences in a non-human primate: Common marmosets provision food altruistically. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 19762–19766. doi:10.1073/pnas.0710310104Google Scholar
  13. Cadieu, N., & Cadieu, J.-C. (2004). The influence of free interactions and partner familiarity on social transmission in the young canary. Animal Behaviour, 67, 1051–1057.Google Scholar
  14. Cadieu, N., Fruchard, S., & Cadieu, J.-C. (2010). Innovative individuals are not always the best demonstrators: Feeding innovations and social transmission in Serinus canaria. PLoS ONE, 5, e8841. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0008841PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Cadieu, N., Winterton, P., & Cadieu, J.-C. (2008). Social transmission of food handling in the context of triadic interactions between adults and young canaries (Serinus canaria). Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology, 62, 795–804. doi:10.1007/s00265-007-0505-0Google Scholar
  16. Chapman, B. B., Ward, A. J. W., & Krause, J. (2008). Schooling and learning: Early social environment predicts social learning ability in the guppy Poecilia reticulata. Animal Behaviour, 76, 923–929.Google Scholar
  17. Corp, N., & Byrne, R. W. (2002). The ontogeny of manual skill in wild chimpanzees: Evidence from feeding on the fruit of the Saba florida. Behaviour, 139, 137–168.Google Scholar
  18. Coussi-Korbel, S., & Fragaszy, D. M. (1995). On the relation between social dynamics and social learning. Animal Behaviour, 50, 1441–1453.Google Scholar
  19. Cunningham, M. A., & Baker, M. C. (1983). Vocal learning in whitecrowned sparrows: Sensitive phase and song dialects. Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology, 13, 259–269.Google Scholar
  20. Day, R. L., MacDonald, T., Brown, C., Laland, K. N., & Reader, S. M. (2001). Interactions between shoal size and conformity in guppy social foraging. Animal Behaviour, 62, 917–925.Google Scholar
  21. de Waal, F. B. M. (2001). The ape and the sushi master. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  22. de Waal, F. B. M., & Bonnie, K. E. (2009). In tune with others: The social side of social learning. In K. N. Laland & B. G. Galef (Eds.), The question of animal culture (pp. 19–40). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  23. Dindo, M., Thierry, B., & Whiten, A. (2008). Social diffusion of novel foraging methods in brown capuchins (Cebus apella). Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 275, 187–193.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Dindo, M., Whiten, A., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2009). In-group conformity sustains different foraging traditions in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). PLoS ONE, 4, e7858. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0007858PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Drea, C., & Wallen, K. (1999). Low-status monkeys “play dumb” when learning in mixed social groups. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 96, 12965–12969.Google Scholar
  26. Efferson, C., Lalive, R., Richerson, P. J., McElreath, R., & Lubell, M. (2008). Conformists and mavericks: The empirics of frequency-dependent cultural transmission. Evolution & Human Behavior, 29, 56–64.Google Scholar
  27. Franz, M., & Nunn, C. L. (2009). Network-based diffusion analysis: A new method for detecting social learning. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 276, 1829–1836.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Franz, M., & Nunn, C. L. (2010). Investigating the impact of observa tion errors on the statistical performance of network based diffusion analysis. Learning & Behavior, 38, 235–242.Google Scholar
  29. Galef, B. G., Jr. (1976). Social transmission of acquired behavior: A discussion of tradition and social learning in vertebrates. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 3, 77–100.Google Scholar
  30. Galef, B. G., Jr. (1996). Social enhancement of food preferences in Norway rats: A brief review. In C. M. Heyes & B. G. Galef, Jr. (Eds.), Social learning and imitation: The roots of culture (pp. 49–64). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  31. Galef, B. G., Jr. (2003). Traditional behaviors of brown and black rats (R. norvegicus and R. rattus). In S. Perry & D. Fragaszy (Eds.), The biology of traditions: Models and evidence (pp. 159–186). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  32. Galef, B. G., Jr., & Clark, M. M. (1971). Social factors in the poison avoidance and feeding behavior of wild and domesticated rat pups. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 75, 341–357.Google Scholar
  33. Galef, B. G., Jr., Rudolf, B., Whiskin, E. E., Choleris, E., Mainardi, M., & Valsecchi, P. (1998). Familiarity and relatedness: Effects on social learning about foods by Norway rats and Mongolian gerbils. Animal Learning & Behavior, 26, 448–454.Google Scholar
  34. Galef, B. G., Jr., & Whiskin, E. E. (2004). Effects of environmental stability and demonstrator age on social learning of food preferences by young Norway rats. Animal Behaviour, 68, 897–902.Google Scholar
  35. Galef, B. G., Jr., & Whiskin, E. E. (2008). Effectiveness of familiar kin and unfamiliar nonkin demonstrator rats in altering food choices of their observers. Animal Behaviour, 76, 1381–1388.Google Scholar
  36. Göth, A., & Evans, C. S. (2005). Life history and social learning: Megapode chicks fail to acquire feeding preferences from conspecifics. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119, 381–386.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Hayashi, M., Mizuno, Y., & Matsuzawa, T. (2005). How does stonetool use emerge? Introduction of stones and nuts to naïve chimpanzees in captivity. Primates, 46, 91–102.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred status as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution & Human Behavior, 22, 165–196.Google Scholar
  39. Herzog, H. A., Bentley, R. A., & Hahn, M. W. (2004). Random drift and large shifts in popularity of dog breeds. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 271, S353-S356.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Hoffer, E. (1955). The passionate state of mind: And other aphorisms. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
  41. Hohmann, G., & Fruth, B. (2003). Culture in bonobos? Between-species and within-species variation in behavior. Current Anthropology, 44, 563–571. doi:10.1086/377649Google Scholar
  42. Holzhaider, J. C., Hunt, G. R., & Gray, R. D. (2010a). The development of pandanus tool manufacture in wild New Caledonian crows. Behaviour, 147, 553–586.Google Scholar
  43. Holzhaider, J. C., Hunt, G. R., & Gray, R. D. (2010b). Social learning in New Caledonian crows. Learning & Behavior, 38, 206–219.Google Scholar
  44. Hoppitt, W., Kandler, A., Kendal, J. R., & Laland, K. N. (2010). The effect of task structure on diffusion dynamics: Implications for diffusion curve and network-based analyses. Learning & Behavior, 38, 243–251.Google Scholar
  45. Hoppitt, W., & Laland, K. N. (2008). Social processes influencing learning in animals: A review of the evidence. Advances in the Study of Behaviour, 38, 105–165.Google Scholar
  46. Horner, V. (in press). The cultural mind of chimpanzees: How social tolerance can shape the transmission of culture. In E. V. Lonsdorf, S. R. Ross, & T. Matsuzawa (Eds.), The mind of the chimpanzee: Ecological and experimental perspectives. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  47. Horner, V., Proctor, D., Bonnie, K. E., Whiten, A., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2010). Prestige affects learning in chimpanzees. PLoS ONE, 5 (5), e10625. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0010625PubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Horner, V., Whiten, A., Flynn, E., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2006). Faithful replication of foraging techniques along cultural transmission chains by chimpanzees and children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103, 13878–13883.Google Scholar
  49. Huffman, M. A. (1996). Acquisition of innovative cultural behaviors in nonhuman primates: A case study of stone handling, a socially transmitted behavior in Japanese macaques. In C. M. Heyes & B. G. Galef, Jr. (Eds.), Social learning in animals: The roots of culture (pp. 267–289). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  50. Humle, T. (in press). How are army ants shedding new light on culture in chimpanzees? In E. V. Lonsdorf, S. R. Ross, & T. Matsuzawa (Eds.), The mind of the chimpanzee: Ecological and experimental perspectives. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  51. Humle, T., Snowdon, C. T., & Matsuzawa, T. (2009). Social influences on ant-dipping acquisition in the wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) of Bossou, Guinea, West Africa. Animal Cognition, 12 (Suppl. 1), S37-S48.Google Scholar
  52. Inoue-Nakamura, N., & Matsuzawa, T. (1997). Development of stone tool use by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 111, 159–173.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Jaeggi, A. V., Dunkel, L. P., Van Noordwijk, M. A., Wich, S. A., Sura, A. A. L., & van Schaik, C. P. (2009). Social learning of diet and foraging skills by wild immature Bornean orangutans: Implications for culture. American Journal of Primatology, 72, 62–71.Google Scholar
  54. Katz, M., & Lachlan, R. F. (2003). Social learning of food types in zebra finches (Taenopygia guttata) is directed by demonstrator sex and feeding activity. Animal Cognition, 6, 11–16. doi:10.1007/s10071 -003-0158-yPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Kawai, M. (1965). Newly-acquired pre-cultural behavior of the natural troop of Japanese monkeys on Koshima islet. Primates, 6, 1–30.Google Scholar
  56. Kendal, R. L., Coolen, I., & Laland, K. N. (2004). The role of conformity in foraging when personal and social information conflict. Behavioral Ecology, 15, 269–277.Google Scholar
  57. Kendal, R. L., Coolen, I., & Laland, K. N. (2009). Adaptive tradeoffs in the use of social and personal information. In R. Dukas & J. Ratcliffe (Eds.), Cognitive ecology: The evolutionary ecology of learning, memory and information use (pp. 249–271). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  58. Kendal, R. L., Custance, D. M., Kendal, J. R., Vale, G., Stoinski, T. S., Rakotomalala, N. L., & Rasamimanana, H. (2010). Evidence for social learning in wild lemurs (Lemur catta). Learning & Behavior, 38, 220–234.Google Scholar
  59. Kendal, R. L., Kendal, J. R., Hoppitt, W., & Laland, K. N. (2009). Identifying social learning in animal populations: A new “option-bias” method. PLoS ONE, 4, e6541. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0006541Google Scholar
  60. Kenward, B., Rutz, C., Weir, A. A. S., & Kacelnik, A. (2006). Development of tool use in New Caledonian crows: Inherited action patterns and social influences. Animal Behaviour, 72, 1329–1343. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.04.007Google Scholar
  61. Kroodsma, D. E. (1978). Aspects of learning in the ontogeny of birdsong: Where, from whom, when, how many, which and how accurately? In G. Burghardt & M. Bekoff (Eds.), Ontogeny of behaviour (pp. 215–230). New York: Garland.Google Scholar
  62. Kroodsma, D. E., Baker, M. C., Baptista, L. F., & Petrinovich, L. (1985). Vocal “dialects” in Nuttall’s white-crowned sparrow. Current Ornithology, 2, 103–133.Google Scholar
  63. Laland, K. N. (2004). Social learning strategies. Learning & Behavior, 32, 4–14.Google Scholar
  64. Laland, K. N., & Hoppitt, W. J. E. (2003). Do animals have culture? Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, 150–159.Google Scholar
  65. Laland, K. N., & Janik, V. M. (2006). The animal cultures debate. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 21, 542–547.Google Scholar
  66. Leca, J., Gunst, N., & Huffman, M. A. (2007). Japanese macaque cultures: Inter- and intra-troop behavioural variability of stone handling patterns across 10 troops. Behaviour, 144, 251–281.Google Scholar
  67. Lefebvre, L., & Bouchard, J. (2003). Social learning about food in birds. In D. M. Fragaszy & S. Perry (Eds.), The biology of traditions (pp. 94–126). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  68. Lonsdorf, E. V. (2005). Sex differences in the development of termitefishing skills in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) of Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Animal Behaviour, 70, 673–683.Google Scholar
  69. Lonsdorf, E. V. (2006). The role of the mother in the acquisition of tooluse skills in wild chimpanzees. Animal Cognition, 9, 36–46.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  70. Lonsdorf, E. V., Eberly, L. E., & Pusey, A. E. (2004). Sex differences in learning in chimpanzees. Nature, 428, 715–716.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. Lonsdorf, E. V., Ross, S. R., Linick, S. A., Milstein, M. S., & Melber, T. N. (2009). An experimental, comparative investigation of tool use in chimpanzees and gorillas. Animal Behaviour, 77, 1119–1126. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.01.020Google Scholar
  72. Lycett, S. J. (2010). The importance of history in definitions of cul ture: Implications from phylogenetic approaches to the study of social learning in chimpanzees. Learning & Behavior, 38, 252–264.Google Scholar
  73. Lycett, S. J., Collard, M., & McGrew, W. C. (2007). Phylogenetic analyses of behavior support existence of culture among wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 17588–17592.Google Scholar
  74. Lycett, S. J., Collard, M., & McGrew, W. C. (2010). Are behavioral differences among wild chimpanzee communities genetic or cultural? An assessment using tool-use data and phylogenetic methods. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 142, 461–467. doi:10.1002/ajpa.21249PubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. Marler, P., & Tamura, M. (1962). Song “dialects” in three populations of white-crowned sparrows. Condor, 64, 368–377.Google Scholar
  76. Mazur, R., & Seher, V. (2008). Socially learned foraging behavior in wild black bears, Ursus americanus. Animal Behaviour, 75, 1503–1508. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.10.027Google Scholar
  77. McGrew, W. C., Ham, R. M., White, L. T. J., Tutin, C. E. G., & Fernandez, M. (1997). Why don’t chimpanzees in Gabon crack nuts? International Journal of Primatology, 18, 353–374.Google Scholar
  78. Nakamura, M. (2002). Grooming-hand-clasp in Mahale M group chimpanzees: Implication for culture in social behaviours. In C. Boesch, G. Hohmann, & L. F. Marchant (Eds.), Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos (pp. 71–83). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  79. Nicol, C. J. (2004). Development, direction, and damage limitation: Social learning in domestic fowl. Learning & Behavior, 32, 72–81.Google Scholar
  80. Nicol, C. J., & Pope, S. J. (1999). The effects of demonstrator social status and prior foraging success on social learning in laying hens. Animal Behaviour, 57, 163–171.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  81. Nottebohm, F. (1970). Ontogeny of bird song. Science, 167, 950–956. doi:10.1126/science.167.3920.950PubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. Ottoni, E. B., de Resende, B. D., & Izar, P. (2005). Watching the best nutcrackers: What capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) know about others’ tool-using skills. Animal Cognition, 8, 215–219.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  83. Ottoni, E. B., & Izar, P. (2008). Capuchin monkey tool use: Overview and implications. Evolutionary Anthropology, 17, 171–178. doi:10.1002/evan.20185Google Scholar
  84. Perry, S. (2009). Conformism in the food processing techniques of white-faced capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus). Animal Cognition, 12, 705–716. doi:10.1007/s10071-009-0230-3PubMedGoogle Scholar
  85. Perry, S., Baker, M., Fedigan, L., Gros-Louis, J., Jack, K., MacKinnon, K., et al. (2003). Social conventions in wild white-faced capuchin monkeys: Evidence for traditions in a neotropical primate. Current Anthropology, 44, 241–268. doi:10.1007/s10071-009-0230-3Google Scholar
  86. Price, E. E., & Caldwell, C. A. (2007). Artificially generated cultural variation between two groups of captive monkeys, Colobus guereza kikuyuensis. Behavioral Processes, 74, 13–20. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2006.09.003Google Scholar
  87. Price, E. E., Lambeth, S. P., Schapiro, S. J., & Whiten, A. (2009). A potent effect of observational learning on chimpanzee tool construction. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 276, 3377–3383. doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.0640PubMedGoogle Scholar
  88. Rapaport, L. M., & Brown, G. R. (2008). Social influences on foraging behaviour in young non-human primates: Learning what, where, and how to eat. Evolutionary Anthropology, 17, 189–201. doi:10.1002/evan.20180Google Scholar
  89. Reader, S. M., & Biro, D. (2010). Experimental identification of social learning in wild animals. Learning & Behavior, 38, 265–283.Google Scholar
  90. Rendell, L. E., & Whitehead, H. (2001). Culture in whales and dolphins. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 24, 309–382.Google Scholar
  91. Ross, S. R., Milstein, M. S., Calcutt, S. E., & Lonsdorf, E. V. (2010). Preliminary assessment of methods used to demonstrate nut cracking behavior to five captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  92. Sanvito, S., Galimberti, F., & Miller, E. H. (2007). Observational evidences of vocal learning in southern elephant seals: A longitudinal study. Ethology, 113, 137–146. doi:10.1111/j.1439 -0310.2006.01306.xGoogle Scholar
  93. Sargeant, B. I., & Mann, J. (2009). Developmental evidence for foraging traditions in wild bottlenose dolphins. Animal Behaviour, 78, 715–721. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.05.037Google Scholar
  94. Scheid, C., Range, F., & Bugnyar, T. (2007). When, what, and whom to watch? Quantifying attention in ravens (Corvus corax) and jackdaws (Corvus monedula). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 121, 380–386. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.121.4.380PubMedGoogle Scholar
  95. Schwab, C., Bugnyar, T., Schloegl, C., & Kotrschal, K. (2008). Enhanced social learning between siblings in common ravens, Corvus corax. Animal Behaviour, 75, 501–508. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.06.006Google Scholar
  96. Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (1986). Vocal development in vervet monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 34, 1640–1658.Google Scholar
  97. Shettleworth, S. J. (1998). Cognition, evolution, and behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  98. Smith, V. A., King, A. P., & West, M. J. (2002). The context of social learning: Association patterns in a captive flock of brown-headed cowbirds. Animal Behaviour, 63, 23–35.Google Scholar
  99. Swaney, W., Kendal, J., Capon, H., Brown, C., & Laland, K. N. (2001). Familiarity facilitates social learning of foraging behavior in the guppy. Animal Behaviour, 62, 591–598. doi:10.1006/anbe.2001.1788Google Scholar
  100. Valsecchi, P., Choleris, E., Moles, A., Guo, C., & Mainardi, M. (1996). Kinship and familiarity as factors affecting social transfer of food preferences in adult Mongolian gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 110, 243–251.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  101. van de Waal, E., Renevey, N., Favre, C. M., & Bshary, R. (2010). Selective attention to philopatric models causes directed social learning in wild vervet monkeys. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 277, 2105–2111. doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.2260PubMedGoogle Scholar
  102. van Schaik, C. P. (2003). Local traditions in orangutans and chimpanzees: Social learning and social tolerance. In S. Perry & D. Fragaszy (Eds.), The biology of traditions: Models and evidence (pp. 297–328). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  103. van Schaik, C. P., Ancrenaz, M., Borgen, G., Galdikas, B., Knott, C. D., Singleton, I., et al. (2003). Orangutan cultures and the evolution of material culture. Science, 299, 102–105. doi:10.1126/science.1078004PubMedGoogle Scholar
  104. van Schaik, C. P., Deaner, R. O., & Merrill, M. Y. (1999). The conditions for tool use in primates: Implications for the evolution of material culture. Journal of Human Evolution, 36, 719–741.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  105. West, M. J., King, A. P., & White, D. J. (2003). Discovering culture in birds: The role of learning and development. In F. B. M. de Waal & P. L. Tyack (Eds.), Animal social complexity: Intelligence, culture, and individualized societies (pp. 470–492). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  106. Wey, T., Blumstein, D. T., Shen, W., & Jordan, F. (2008). Social network analysis of animal behaviour: A promising tool for the study of sociality. Animal Behaviour, 75, 333–344.Google Scholar
  107. Whitehead, H. (2010). Conserving and managing animals that learn socially and share cultures. Learning & Behavior, 38, 329–336.Google Scholar
  108. Whitehead, H., & Rendell, L. (2004). Movements, habitat use and feeding success of cultural clans of South Pacific sperm whales. Journal of Animal Ecology, 73, 190–196.Google Scholar
  109. Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., et al. (1999). Culture in chimpanzees. Nature, 399, 682–685.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  110. Whiten, A., & Ham, R. (1992). On the nature and evolution of imitation in the animal kingdom: Reappraisal of a century of research. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 21, 239–283.Google Scholar
  111. Whiten, A., Horner, V., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2005). Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees. Nature, 437, 737–740.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  112. Whiten, A., Horner, V., Litchfield, C. A., & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2004). How do apes ape? Learning & Behavior, 32, 36–52.Google Scholar
  113. Whiten, A., & Mesoudi, A. (2008). Establishing an experimental science of culture: Animal social diffusion experiments. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 363, 3477–3488. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0134Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Lester E. Fisher Center for the Study and Conservation of ApesChicago
  2. 2.University of ChicagoChicago
  3. 3.Beloit CollegeBeloit

Personalised recommendations