Skip to main content

The derived generalization of thought suppression

Abstract

Thought suppression appears to be a relatively ineffective and even counterproductive strategy for dealing with unwanted thoughts. However, the psychological processes responsible for unsuccessful suppression are still underspecified. One process that may be implicated is derived stimulus relations, which may underlie the formation of unintentional relations that act to hamper suppression attempts. To test this prediction, participants were trained and tested for the formation of three derived equivalence relations using a match-to-sample procedure. Subsequently, they were instructed to suppress all thoughts of a particular target word that was a member of one of the three relations and were also allowed to selectively remove words that appeared on a computer screen in front of them by pressing the space bar. Results showed, as predicted, that participants not only removed the to-be-suppressed stimulus, but also removed words in derived relations with that stimulus, thus showing transformation of suppression/interference functions via derived equivalence. The theoretical implications of this demonstration, including its potential as a model for a key psychological process involved in unsuccessful thought suppression, are discussed.

References

  • Augustson, E. M., & Dougher, M. J. (1997). The transfer of avoidance evoking functions through stimulus equivalence classes. Journal of Behavior Therapy & Experimental Psychiatry, 28, 181–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, D. (1989). Behavior-behavior analysis, human schedule performance, and radical behaviorism. Psychological Record, 39, 339–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, D., & Browne, M., Smeets, P. [M.], & Roche, B. (1995). A transfer of functions and a conditional transfer of functions through equivalence relations in three-to six-year-old children. Psychological Record, 45, 405–430.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, D., & Keenan, M. (1993). A transfer of functions through derived arbitrary and nonarbitrary stimulus relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 59, 61–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Cochrane, A., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Stewart, I., & McHugh, L. (2004). Psychological acceptance: Experimental analyses and theoretical interpretations. International Journal of Psychology & Psychological Therapy, 4, 517–530.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory (2nd ed.). San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beevers, C. G., Wenzlaff, R. M., Hayes, A. M., & Scott, W. D. (1999). Depression and the ironic effects of thought suppression: Therapeutic strategies for improving mental control. Clinical Psychology: Science & Practice, 6, 133–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bond, F. W., Hayes, S. C., Baer, R. A., Carpenter, K. M., Orcutt, H. K., Waltz, T., & Zettle, R. D. (2010). Preliminary psychometric properties of the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire-II: A revised measure of psychological flexibility and acceptance. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Brewin, C. R., Watson, M., McCarthy, S., Hyman, P., & Dayson, D. (1998). Intrusive memories and depression in cancer patients. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 36, 1131–1142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cook, M., & Mineka, S. (1990). Selective associations in the observational conditioning of fear in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 372–389.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dymond, S., & Barnes, D. (1995). A transformation of self-discrimination response functions in accordance with the arbitrarily applicable relations of sameness, more than, and less than. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64, 163–184.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dymond, S., & Barnes, D. (1996). A transformation of self-discrimination response functions in accordance with the arbitrarily applicable relations of sameness and opposition. Psychological Record, 46, 271–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dymond, S., & Rehfeldt, R. (2000). Understanding complex behavior: The transformation of stimulus functions. Behavior Analyst, 23, 239–254.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dymond, S., & Roche, B. (2009). A contemporary behavior analysis of anxiety and avoidance. Behavior Analyst, 32, 7–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dymond, S., Roche, B., Forsyth, J. P., Whelan, R., & Rhoden, J. (2007). Transformation of avoidance response functions in accordance with same and opposite relational frames. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 88, 249–262.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., & Brownstein, A. J. (1986). Mentalism, behavior-behavior relations, and a behavior-analytic view of the purposes of science. Behavior Analyst, 9, 175–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lavy, E. H., & Van den Hout, M. A. (1990). Thought suppression induces intrusions. Behavioural Psychotherapy, 18, 251–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markham, M. R., Dougher, M. J., & Augustson, E. M. (2002). Transfer of operant discrimination and respondent elicitation via emergent relations of compound stimuli. Psychological Record, 52, 325–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • McHugh, L., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2004). Understanding perspective-taking, false belief, and deception from a behavioural perspective. Irish Psychologist, 30, 142–147.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mineka, S., & Tomarken, A. J. (1989). The role of cognitive biases in the origins and maintenance of fear and anxiety disorders. In T. Archer & L.-G. Nilsson (Eds.), Aversion, avoidance, and anxiety: Perspectives on aversively motivated behavior (pp. 195–221). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Najmi, S., & Wegner, D. M. (2008). The gravity of unwanted thoughts: Asymmetric priming effects in thought suppression. Consciousness & Cognition, 17, 114–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rachman, S., & de Silva, P. (1978). Abnormal and normal obsessions. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 16, 233–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rassin, E., Muris, P., Jong, J., & de Bruin, G. (2005). Summoning white bears or letting them free: The influence of the content of control instructions on target thought frequency. Journal of Psychopathology & Behavioral Assessment, 27, 253–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reiss, S. (1980). Pavlovian conditioning and human fear: An expectancy model. Behavior Therapy, 11, 380–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roche, B., & Barnes, D. (1997). A transformation of respondently conditioned stimulus function in accordance with arbitrarily applicable relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 67, 275–301.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roche, B., Barnes-Holmes, D., Smeets, P. M., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & McGeady, S. (2000). Contextual control over the derived transformation of discriminative and sexual arousal functions. Psychological Record, 50, 267–291.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schell, A. M., Dawson, M. E., & Marinkovic, K. (1991). Effects of potentially phobic conditioned stimuli on retention, reconditioning, and extinction of the conditioned skin conductance response. Psychophysiology, 28, 140–153.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1994). Equivalence relations and behavior: A research story. Boston: Authors Cooperative.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M., Kirk, B., & Willson-Morris, M. (1985). Six-member stimulus classes generated by conditional-discrimination procedures. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43, 21–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steele, D. [L.], & Hayes, S. C. (1991). Stimulus equivalence and arbitrarily applicable relational responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 56, 519–555.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walther, E., Nagengast, B., & Trasselli, C. (2005). Evaluative conditioning in social psychology: Facts and speculations. Cognition & Emotion, 19, 175–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M. (1989). White bears and other unwanted thoughts: Suppression, obsession, and the psychology of mental control. New York: Viking.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M., & Erber, R. (1992). The hyperaccessibility of suppressed thoughts. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 63, 903–912.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M., & Gold, D. B. (1995). Fanning old flames: Emotional and cognitive effects of suppressing thoughts of a past relationship. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 68, 782–792.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., III, & White, T. L. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 53, 5–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Knutson, B., & McMahon, S. R. (1991). Polluting the stream of consciousness: The effect of thought suppression on the mind’s environment. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 15, 141–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M., & Zanakos, S. (1994). Chronic thought suppression. Journal of Personality, 62, 615–640.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whelan, R., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2004). The transformation of consequential functions in accordance with the relational frames of same and opposite. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 82, 177–195.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wulfert, E., & Hayes, S. C. (1988). Transfer of a conditional ordering response through conditional equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 125–144.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nic Hooper.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hooper, N., Saunders, J. & McHugh, L. The derived generalization of thought suppression. Learning & Behavior 38, 160–168 (2010). https://doi.org/10.3758/LB.38.2.160

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/LB.38.2.160

Keywords

  • Target Word
  • Comparison Stimulus
  • Unrelated Word
  • Present Stud
  • Thought Suppression