Behavior Research Methods

, Volume 42, Issue 4, pp 987–991 | Cite as

Leipzig Affective Norms for German: A reliability study

Article

Abstract

To facilitate investigations of verbal emotional processing, we introduce the Leipzig Affective Norms for German (LANG), a list of 1,000 German nouns that have been rated for emotional valence, arousal, and concreteness. A critical factor regarding the quality of normative word data is their reliability. We therefore acquired ratings from a sample that was tested twice, with an interval of 2 years, to calculate test—retest reliability. Furthermore, we recruited a second sample to test reliability across independent samples. The results show (1) the typical quadratic relation of valence and arousal, replicating previous data, (2) very high test—retest reliability (>.95), and (3) high correlations between the two samples (>.85). Because the range of ratings was also very high, we provide a comprehensive set of words with reliable affective norms, which makes it possible to select highly controlled subsamples varying in emotional status. The database is available as a supplement for this article at http://brm.psychonomic-journals.org/content/supplemental.

Supplementary material

Kanske-BRM-2010.zip (77 kb)
Supplementary material, approximately 340 KB.

References

  1. Altarriba, J. B., Bauer, L. M., & Benvenuto, C. (1999). Concreteness, context availability, and imageability ratings and word associations for abstract, concrete, and emotion words. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 31, 578–602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Besnier, N., Richard, F., Zendjidjian, X., Kaladjian, A., Mazzola-Pomietto, P., Adida, M., & Azorin, J. M. (2009). Stroop and emotional Stroop interference in unaffected relatives of patients with schizophrenic and bipolar disorders: Distinct markers of vulnerability? World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 10, 809–818.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: The Self-Assessment Manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy & Experimental Psychiatry, 25, 49–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1999). Affective Norms for English Words (ANEW): Stimuli, instruction manual, and affective ratings (Tech. Rep. C-1). Gainesville: University of Florida, Center for Research in Psychophysiology.Google Scholar
  5. Dillon, D. G., Ritchey, M., Johnson, B. D., & LaBar, K. S. (2007). Dissociable effects of conscious emotion regulation strategies on explicit and implicit memory. Emotion, 7, 354–365.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Duden (2000). Die deutsche Rechtschreibung. Mannheim: Author.Google Scholar
  7. Eilola, T. M., & Havelka, J. (2010). Affective norms for 210 British English and Finnish nouns. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 134–140.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Eviatar, Z., & Zaidel, E. (1991). The effects of word length and emotionality on hemispheric contribution to lexical decision. Neuropsychologia, 29, 415–428.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Hager, W., & Hasselhorn, M. (1994). Handbuch deutschsprachiger Wortnormen. Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
  10. Herrington, J. D., Heller, W., Mohanty, A., Engels, A. S., Banich, M. T., Webb, A. G., et al. (2010). Localization of asymmetric brain function in emotion and depression. Psychophysiology, 47, 442–454.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Hodes, R. L., Cook, E. W., & Lang, P. J. (1985). Individual differences in autonomic response: Conditioned association or conditioned fear? Psychophysiology, 22, 545–560.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Jänsch, C., Harmer, C., & Cooper, M. J. (2009). Emotional processing in women with anorexia nervosa and in healthy volunteers. Eating Behaviors, 10, 184–191.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Kanske, P., & Kotz, S. A. (2007). Concreteness in emotional words: ERP evidence from a hemifield study. Brain Research, 1148, 138–148.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Kanske, P., & Kotz, S. A. (in press). Emotion triggers executive attention: Anterior cingulate cortex and amygdala responses to emotional words in a conflict task. Human Brain Mapping. doi:10.1002/hbm.21012.Google Scholar
  15. Kuchinke, L., Jacobs, A. M., Võ, M. L., Conrad, M., Grubich, C., & Herrmann, M. (2006). Modulation of prefrontal cortex activation by emotional words in recognition memory. NeuroReport, 17, 1037–1041.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Lahl, O., Göritz, A. S., Pietrowsky, R., & Rosenberg, J. (2009). Using the World-Wide Web to obtain large-scale word norms: 190,212 ratings on a set of 2,654 German nouns. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 13–19.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Maddock, R. J., Garrett, A. S., & Buonocore, M. H. (2003). Posterior cingulate cortex activation by emotional words: fMRI evidence from a valence decision task. Human Brain Mapping, 18, 30–41.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Mathewson, K. J., Arnell, K. M., & Mansfield, C. A. (2008). Capturing and holding attention: The impact of emotional words in rapid serial visual presentation. Memory & Cognition, 36, 182–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Nakic, M., Smith, B. W., Busis, S., Vythilingam, M., & Blair, J. R. (2006). The impact of affect and frequency on lexical decision: The role of the amygdala and inferior frontal cortex. NeuroImage, 31, 1752–1761.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–98.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Ortigue, S., Michel, C. M., Murray, M. M., Mohr, C., Carbonnel, S., & Landis, T. (2004). Electrical neuroimaging reveals early generator modulation to emotional words. NeuroImage, 21, 1242–1251.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Osaka, N., Osaka, M., Morishita, M., Kondo, H., & Fukuyama, H. (2004). A word expressing affective pain activates the anterior cingulate cortex in the human brain: An fMRI study. Behavioural Brain Research, 153, 123–127.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Osterhout, L. (1997). On the brain response to syntactic anomalies: Manipulations of word position and word class reveal individual differences. Brain & Language, 59, 494–522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Perani, D., Cappa, S. F., Schnur, T., Tettamanti, M., Collina, S., Rosa, M. M., & Fazio, F. (1999). The neural correlates of verb and noun processing: A PET study. Brain, 122, 2337–2344.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Redondo, J., Fraga, I., Padrón, I., & Comesaña, M. (2007). The Spanish adaptation of ANEW (Affective Norms for English Words). Behavior Research Methods, 39, 600–605.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Rodway, P., Wright, L., & Hardie, S. (2003). The valence-specific laterality effect in free viewing conditions: The influence of sex, handedness, and response bias. Brain & Cognition, 53, 452–463.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Scott, G. G., O’Donnell, P. J., Leuthold, H., & Sereno, S. C. (2009). Early emotion word processing: Evidence from event-related potentials. Biological Psychology, 80, 95–104.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Sim, T.-C., & Martinez, C. (2005). Emotion words are remembered better in the left ear. Laterality, 10, 149–159.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Stevenson, R. A., Mikels, J. A., & James, T. W. (2007). Characterization of the Affective Norms for English Words by discrete emotional categories. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 1020–1024.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Stormark, K. M., Nordby, H., & Hugdahl, K. (1995). Attentional shifts to emotionally charged cues: Behavioral and ERP data. Cognition & Emotion, 9, 507–523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. van Hooff, J. C., Dietz, K. C., Sharma, D., & Bowman, H. (2008). Neural correlates of intrusion of emotion words in a modified Stroop task. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 67, 23–34.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Võ, M. L., Conrad, M., Kuchinke, L., Urton, K., Hofmann, M. J., & Jacobs, A. M. (2009). The Berlin Affective Word List Reloaded (BAWL-R). Behavior Research Methods, 41, 534–538.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Võ, M. L., Jacobs, A. M., & Conrad, M. (2006). Cross-validating the Berlin Affective Word List. Behavior Research Methods, 38, 606–609.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Wentura, D. (2000). Dissociative affective and associative priming effects in the lexical decision task: Yes versus no responses to word targets reveal evaluative judgment tendencies. Experimental Psychology, 26, 456–469.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain SciencesLeipzigGermany
  2. 2.Department of Cognitive and Clinical NeuroscienceCentral Institute of Mental HealthMannheimGermany

Personalised recommendations