Behavior Research Methods

, Volume 42, Issue 1, pp 168–178 | Cite as

Inferring intentions from biological motion: A stimulus set of point-light communicative interactions

  • Valeria Manera
  • Ben Schouten
  • Cristina Becchio
  • Bruno G. Bara
  • Karl Verfaillie
Article

Abstract

We present the first database of communicative interactions reproduced through point-light displays (Communicative Interaction Database). The database contains 20 communicative interactions performed by male and by female couples. For each action, we provide movie files from four different viewpoints, as well as text files with the 3-D spatial coordinates of the point lights, allowing researchers to construct customized versions. By including various types of actions performed with different social motives, the database contains a diverse sample of nonconventional communicative gestures. Normative data collected to assess the recognizability of the stimuli suggest that, for most action stimuli, information in point-light displays is sufficient for clear recognition of the action as communicative, as well as for identification of the specific communicative gesture performed by the actor. The full set of stimuli may be downloaded from http://brm.psychonomic-journals.org/ content/supplemental and from http://ppw.kuleuven.be/labexppsy/lepSite/resources/CID.rar.

Supplementary material

Manera-BRM-2010.zip (50.8 mb)
Supplementary material, approximately 340 KB.

References

  1. Atkinson, A. P., Dittrich, W. H., Gemmell, A. J., & Young, A. W. (2004). Emotion perception from dynamic and static body expressions in point-light and full-light displays. Perception, 33, 717–746. doi:10.1068/p5096PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Autodesk, Inc. (1997). Autodesk 3D Studio Max (Release 2) [Computer software]. Sausalito, CA: Author.Google Scholar
  3. Autodesk, Inc. (1998). Character Studio Max (Release 2) [Computer software]. Sausalito, CA: Author.Google Scholar
  4. Bara, B. G. (2010). Cognitive pragmatics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  5. Becchio, C., Sartori, L., Bulgheroni, M., & Castiello, U. (2008). The case of Dr. Jekyll and Mr. Hyde: A kinematic study on social intention. Consciousness & Cognition, 17, 557–564. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2007.03.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Blakemore, S.-J., & Decety, J. (2001). From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 561–567. doi:10.1038/35086023PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Brooks, A., Schouten, B., Troje, N., Verfaillie, K., Blanke, O., & van der Zwan, R. (2008). Correlated changes in perceptions of the gender and orientation of ambiguous biological motion figures. Current Biology, 18, R728-R729. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.06.054PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Clarke, T. J., Bradshaw, M. F., Field, D. T., Hampson, S. E., & Rose, D. (2005). The perception of emotion from body movement in point-light displays of interpersonal dialogue. Perception, 34, 1171–1180. doi:10.1068/p5203PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Cohen, D. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educational & Psychological Measurement, 20, 37–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cutting, J. E., & Kozlowski, L. T. (1977). Recognizing friends by their walk: Gait perception without familiarity cues. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 9, 353–356.Google Scholar
  11. Daems, A., & Verfaillie, K. (1999). Viewpoint-dependent priming effects in the perception of human actions and body postures. Visual Cognition, 6, 665–693. doi:10.1080/135062899394894CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Decety, J., & Sommerville, J. A. (2003). Shared representations between self and other: A social cognitive neuroscience view. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 527–533. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.004PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Dekeyser, M., Verfaillie, K., & Vanrie, J. (2002). Creating stimuli for the study of biological-motion perception. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 34, 375–382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Dittrich, W. H. (1993). Action categories and the perception of biological motion. Perception, 22, 15–22. doi:10.1068/p220015PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dittrich, W. H., Troscianko, T., Lea, S., & Morgan, D. (1996). Perception of emotion from dynamic point-light displays represented in dance. Perception, 25, 727–738. doi:10.1068/p250727PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Grice, H. P. (1989). Studies in the way of words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  17. Grossman, E., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Pickens, D., Morgan, V., Neighbor, G., & Blake, R. (2000). Brain areas involved in perception of biological motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 711–720. doi:10.1162/089892900562417PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hoenkamp, E. (1978). Perceptual cues that determine the labelling of human gait. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 4, 59–69.Google Scholar
  19. Jacob, P. (2009). The tuning-fork model of human social cognition: A critique. Consciousness & Cognition, 18, 229–243. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2008.05.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Jacob, P., & Jeannerod, M. (2005). The motor theory of social cognition: A critique. Trends in Cognitive Neurosciences, 9, 21–25. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.11.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Kilner, J. M., Marchant, J. L., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Modulation of the mirror system by social relevance. Social Cognitive & Affective Neuroscience, 1, 143–148. doi:10.1093/scan/nsl017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Kozlowski, L. T., & Cutting, J. E. (1977). Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light display. Perception & Psychophysics, 21, 575–580.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33, 159–174.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Langton, S. R. H., Watt, R. J., & Bruce, V. (2000). Do the eyes have it? Cues to the direction of social attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 50–59. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01436-9PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Social cognitive neuroscience: A review of core processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 259–289. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085654PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Loula, F., Prasad, S., Harber, K., & Shiffrar, M. (2005). Recognizing people from their movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 31, 210–220. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.31.1.210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Ma, Y., Paterson, H. M., & Pollick, F. E. (2006). A motion capture library for the study of identity, gender, and emotion perception from biological motion. Behavior Research Methods, 38, 134–141.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Montpare, J. M., & Zebrowitz-McArthur, L. (1988). Impressions of people created by age-related qualities of their gaits. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 55, 547–556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Ochsner, K. N., & Lieberman, M. D. (2001). The emergence of social cognitive neuroscience. American Psychologist, 56, 717–734. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.56.9.717PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Olofsson, U., Nyberg, L., & Nilsson, L. G. (1997). Priming and recognition of human motion patterns. Visual Cognition, 4, 373–382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Oxford Metrics, Ltd. (1997). Body Builder Version 3.5 [Computer software]. Oxford: Author.Google Scholar
  32. Perrett, D. I., Harries, M. H., Bevan, R., Thomas, S., Benson, P. J., Mistlin, A. J., et al. (1989). Frameworks of analysis for the neural representation of animate objects and actions. Journal of Experimental Biology, 146, 87–113.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Peuskens, H., Vanrie, J., Verfaillie, K., & Orban, G. A. (2005). Specificity of regions processing biological motion. European Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 2864–2875. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04106.xPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Pierno, A., Becchio, C., Wall, M. B., Smith, A. T., Turella, L., & Castiello, U. (2006). When gaze turns into grasp. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 2130–2137. doi:10.1162/jocn.2006.18.12.2130PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Pollick, F. E., Fidopiastis, C., & Braden, V. (2001). Recognizing the style of spatially exaggerated tennis serves. Perception, 30, 323–338. doi:10.1068/p3064PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Pollick, F. E., Kay, J. W., Heim, K., & Stringer, R. (2005). Gender recognition from point-light walkers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 31, 1247–1265. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.31.6.1247CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Pollick, F. E., Paterson, H. M., Bruderlin, A., & Sanford, A. J. (2001). Perceiving affect from arm movement. Cognition, 82, B51-B61. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00147-0PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Puce, A., & Perrett, D. (2003). Electrophysiology and brain imaging of biological motion. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 358, 435–445. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1221CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Sartori, L., Becchio, C., Bara, B. G., & Castiello, U. (2009). Does the intention to communicate affect action kinematics? Consciousness & Cognition, 18, 766–772.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Saxe, R. (2006). Uniquely human social cognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16, 235–239. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2006.03.001PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Schilbach, L., Wohlschlaeger, A. M., Kraemer, N. C., Newenh, A., Sha, N. J., Fink, G. R., & Vogeley, K. (2005). Being with virtual others: Neural correlates of social interaction. Neuropsychologia, 44, 718–730. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.07.017PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Searle, J. R. (1969). Speech acts. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  44. Searle, J. R. (1979). Expression and meaning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  45. Shipley, T. F., & Brumberg, J. S. (2005). Markerless motion-capture for point-light displays. Available at http://astro.temple.edu/~tshipley/mocap/MarkerlessMoCap.pdf.Google Scholar
  46. Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1995). Relevance: Communication and cognition. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  47. Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 28, 675–735. doi:10.1017/S0140525X05000129Google Scholar
  48. Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78, 705–722. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01025.xPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Troje, N. F. (2002). Decomposing biological motion: A framework for analyses and synthesis of human gait patterns. Journal of Vision, 2, 371–387. doi:10.1167/2.5.2PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Vanrie, J., & Verfaillie, K. (2004). Perception of biological motion: A stimulus set of human point-light actions. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 625–629.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Verfaillie, K. (1993). Orientation-dependent priming effects in the perception of biological motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 19, 992–1013. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.19.5.992CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Verfaillie, K. (2000). Visual perception of human locomotion: Priming effects in direction discrimination. Brain & Cognition, 44, 192–213. doi:10.1006/brcg.2000.1228CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  • Valeria Manera
    • 2
    • 1
  • Ben Schouten
    • 2
  • Cristina Becchio
    • 2
  • Bruno G. Bara
    • 2
  • Karl Verfaillie
    • 1
  1. 1.University of LeuvenLeuvenBelgium
  2. 2.Center for Cognitive Science, Department of PsychologyUniversity of TurinTurinItaly

Personalised recommendations