Advertisement

Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society

, Volume 28, Issue 2, pp 138–140 | Cite as

Effects of noise upon introverts and extroverts

  • Lionel Standing
  • Danny Lynn
  • Katherine Moxness
Article

Abstract

The effects of noise upon arousal, mood, and cognitive performance were examined in two experiments employing selected groups of introverts and extroverts as subjects. In the first experiments, white noise at 60 dB produced higher physiological activation in introverts, but not in extroverts, as predicted from Eysenck’s theory of personality. Likewise, performance in a comprehension task was impaired in introverts but not in extroverts. Extroverts were also shown to display more subjective deactivation under noise, and to have greater habitual tolerance for noisy real-world environments. In the second experiment, exposure to patterned noise increased state anxiety in both introverts and extroverts, but did not affect aggressiveness.

Keywords

Reading Comprehension State Anxiety Quiet Condition Noise Stimulus Rock Music 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Bryant, M. (1973). Noise laws don’t protect the sensitive. New Scientist, 27, 738–740.Google Scholar
  2. Daoussis, L., & McKelvie, S. (1986). Musical preference and the effects of music on a reading comprehension test for extraverts and introverts. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 62, 283–289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Edsell, R. (1976). Anxiety as a function of environmental noise and social interaction. Journal of Psychology, 92, 219–226.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Eysenck, H. (1967). The biological basis of personality. Springfield, IL: Thomas.Google Scholar
  5. Eysenck, H., & Eysenck, S. (1963). Manual for the Eysenck personality inventory. San Diego: Educational & Industrial Testing Service.Google Scholar
  6. Eysenck, M. (1975). Effects of noise, activation level, and response dominance on retrieval from semantic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory, 104, 143–148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Feshbach, S. (1961). Stimulating versus cathartic effects of a vicarious aggressive activity. Journal of Abnormal & Social Psychology, 63, 381–385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Holding, D., & Baker, M. (1988). Toward meaningful noise research. Journal of General Psychology, 114, 395–410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Mueller, C. (1983). Environmental stressors and aggressive behavior. In R. Geen & E. Donnerstein (Eds.), Aggression: Theoretical and empirical reviews (Vol. 2, pp. 51–76). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  10. Smith, C., & Morris, L. (1977). Effects of stimulative and sedative music on cognition and emotional components of anxiety. Psychological Reports, 38, 1187–1193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Spielberger, C., Gorsuch, R., & Lushene, R.(1970). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
  12. Standing, L., & Stace, G. (1980). Effects of environmental noise on anxiety level. Journal of General Psychology, 103, 263–272.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Stanford, R., Angelini, R., & Raphael, A. (1985). Cognition and mood during ganzfeld: Effects of extraversion and noise versus silence. Journal of Parapsychology, 49, 165–191.Google Scholar
  14. Thayer, R. (1967). Measurement of activation through self-report. Psychological Reports, 20, 663–678.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Weinstein, N. (1978). Individual differences in reactions to noise: A longitudinal study in a college dormitory. Journal of Applied Psychology, 63, 458–466.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The psychonomic Society, Inc. 1990

Authors and Affiliations

  • Lionel Standing
    • 1
  • Danny Lynn
    • 1
  • Katherine Moxness
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyBishop’s UniversityLennoxvilleCanada

Personalised recommendations