Advertisement

Memory & Cognition

, Volume 27, Issue 3, pp 494–500 | Cite as

Norms for word lists that create false memories

  • Michael A. Stadler
  • Henry L. Roediger
  • Kathleen B. McDermott
Article

Abstract

Roediger and McDermott (1995) induced false recall and false recognition for words that were not presented in lists. They had subjects study 24 lists of 15 words that were associates of a common word (called the critical target or critical lure) that was not presented in the list. False recall and false recognition of the critical target occurred frequently in response to these lists. The purpose of the current work was to provide a set of normative data for the lists Roediger and McDermott used and for 12 others developed more recently. We tested false recall and false recognition for critical targets from 36 lists. Despite the fact that all lists were constructed to produce false remembering, the diversity in their effectiveness was large—60% or more of subjects falsely recalledwindow andsleep following the appropriate lists, and false recognition for these items was greater than 80%. However, the list generated fromking led to 10% false recall and 27% false recognition. Possible reasons for these wide differences in effectiveness of the lists are discussed. These norms serve as a useful benchmark for designing experiments about false recall and false recognition in this paradigm.

Keywords

Free Recall Serial Position False Memory False Recognition Critical Item 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Bruce, D., &Winograd, E. (1998). Remembering Deese’s 1959 articles: The Zeitgeist, the sociology of science, and false memories.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,5, 615–624.Google Scholar
  2. Cofer, C. N. (1967). Does conceptual organization influence the amount retained in immediate free recall? In B. Kleinmuntz (Ed.),Concepts and the structure of memory (pp. 181–214). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  3. Deese, J. (1959a). Influence of inter-item associative strength upon immediate free recall.Psychological Reports,5, 305–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Deese, J. (1959b). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall.Journal of Experimental Psychology,58, 17–22.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Deese, J. (1965).The structure of associations in language and thought. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press.Google Scholar
  6. Gallo, D. A., Roberts, M. J., &Seamon, J. G. (1997). Remembering words not presented in lists: Can we avoid creating false memories?Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,4, 271–276.Google Scholar
  7. Gardiner, J. M. (1988). Functional aspects of recollective experience.Memory & Cognition,16, 309–313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Mather, M., Henkel, L. A., &Johnson, M. K. (1997). Evaluating characteristics of false memories: Remember/know judgments and memory characteristics questionnaire compared.Memory & Cognition,25, 826–837.Google Scholar
  9. McDermott, K. B. (1995). [Associative lists that induce false memories]. Unpublished materials.Google Scholar
  10. McDermott, K. B. (1996). The persistence of false memories in list recall.Journal of Memory & Language,35, 212–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. McDermott, K. B., &Roediger, H. L., III (1998). False recognition of associates can be resistant to an explicit warning to subjects and an immediate recognition probe.Journal of Memory & Language,39, 508–520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Norman, K. A., &Schacter, D. L. (1997). False recognition in younger and older adults: Exploring the characteristics of illusory memories.Memory & Cognition,25, 838–848.Google Scholar
  13. Payne, D. G., Elie, C. J., Blackwell, J. M., &Neuschatz, J. S. (1996). Memory illusions: Recalling, recognizing, and recollecting events that never occurred.Journal of Memory & Language,35, 261–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Roediger, H. L., III (1996). Memory illusions.Journal of Memory & Language,35, 76–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Roediger, H. L., III, &McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,21, 803–814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Roediger, H. L., III,McDermott, K. B., &Robinson, K. J. (1998). The role of associative processes in creating false memories. In M. A. Conway, S. E. Gathercole, & C. Cornoldi (Eds.),Theories of memory II (pp. 187–246). Hove, U.K.: Psychological Press.Google Scholar
  17. Roediger, H. L., III, &Payne, D. G. (1985). Recall criterion does not affect recall level or hypermnesia: A puzzle for generate/recognize theories.Memory & Cognition,13, 1–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Russell, W. A., &Jenkins, J. J. (1954).The complete Minnesota norms for responses to 100 words from the Kent-Rosanoff Word Association Test. (Tech. Rep. No. 11, Contract N8 ONR 66216, Office of Naval Research). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota.Google Scholar
  19. Schacter, D. L., Verfaellie, M., &Pradere, D. (1996). The neuropsychology of memory illusions: False recall and recognition in amnesic patients.Journal of Memory & Language,35, 319–334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and consciousness.Canadian Psychologist,26, 1–12.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 1999

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of MissouriColumbia
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyWashington UniversitySt., Louis

Personalised recommendations