Advertisement

Memory & Cognition

, Volume 26, Issue 1, pp 3–19 | Cite as

Things learned in early adulthood are remembered best

  • David C. Rubin
  • Tamara A. Rahhal
  • Leonard W. Poon
Article

Abstract

Evidence is reviewed that for older adults the period from 10 to 30 years of age produces recall of the most autobiographical memories, the most vivid memories, and the most important memories. It is the period from which peoples’ favorite films, music, and books come and the period from which they judge the most important world events to have originated. Factual, semantic, general-knowledge, multiple-choice questions about the Academy Awards, the World Series, and current events from this period were answered more accurately by two different groups of 30 older adults tested 10 years apart. A cognitive theory based on the importance of transitions and several noncognitive theories are considered as explanations of this pervasive phenomenon.

Keywords

Early Adulthood Autobiographical Memory Proactive Interference News Story Remote Memory 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Anderson, J. R. (1990).The adaptive character of thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  2. Anderson, J. R., &Schooler, L. J. (1991). Reflections of the environment in memory.Psychological Science,2, 396–408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Atkinson, R. C., &Shiffrin, R. M., (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In K.W. Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.),The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 2, pp. 90–195), New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  4. Bahrick, H. P. (1979). Maintenance of knowledge: Questions about memory we forgot to ask.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,108, 296–308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bahrick, H. P. (1984). Semantic memory content in permastore: Fifty years of memory for Spanish learned in school.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,113, 1–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bartlett, F. C. (1932).Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Belli, R. F., Schuman, H., &Jackson, B. (1997). Autobiographical misremembering: John Dean is not alone.Applied Cognitive Psychology,11, 187–209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Benson, K. A., Jarvi, S. D., Arai, Y., Thielbar, P. R. S., Frye, K. J., &McDonald, B. L. G. (1992). Socio-historical context and autobiographical memories: Variations in the reminiscence phenomenon. In M. A. Conway, D. C. Rubin, H. Spinnler, & W. A. Wagenaar (Eds.),Theoretical perspectives on autobiographical memory (pp. 313–322). Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
  9. Bjork, R. A., &Bjork, E. L. (1992). A new theory of disuse and an old theory of stimulus fluctuation. In A. Healy, S. Kosslyn, & R. Shiffrin (Eds.),From learning processes to cognitive processes: Essays in honor of William K. Estes (Vol. 2, pp. 35–67). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  10. Bornstein, R. F. (1989). Exposure and affect: Overview and metaanalysis of research, 1968–1987.Psychological Bulletin,106, 265–289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Botwinick, J., &Storandt, M. (1974).Memory, related functions and age. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.Google Scholar
  12. Botwinick, J., &Storandt, M. (1980). Recall and recognition of old information in relation to age and sex.Journal of Gerontology,35, 70–76.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Braun, K., & Rubin, D. C. (in press). The spacing effect depends on an encoding deficit, retrieval, and time in working memory: Evidence from once presented words.Memory.Google Scholar
  14. Bruce, D. (1985). The how and why of ecological memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,114, 80–92.Google Scholar
  15. Butters, N., &Cermak, L. S. (1986). A case study of forgetting of autobiographical knowledge: Implications for the study of retrograde amnesia. In D. C. Rubin (Ed.),Autobiographical memory (pp. 253–272). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Carstensen, L. L. (1992). Social and emotional patterns in adulthood: Support for socioemotional selectivity theory.Psychology & Aging,7, 331–338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Carstensen, L. L. (1995). Evidence of a life-span theory of socioemotional selectivity.Current Directions in Psychological Science,4, 151–156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Cerella, J. (1985). Information processing rates in the elderly.Psychological Bulletin,98, 67–83.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Cerella, J. (1991). Age effects may be global, not local: Comment on Fisk and Rogers (1991).Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,120, 215–223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Cerella, J., &Hale, S. (1994). The rise and fall in information-processing rates over the life span.Acta Psychologica,86, 109–197.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Cerella, J., Poon, L.W., &Williams, D. M. (1980). Age and the complexity hypothesis. In L. W. Poon (Ed.),Aging in the 1980s: Psychological issues (pp. 332–340). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Cohen, G., Conway, M. A., &Maylor, E. A. (1994). Flashbulb memories in older adults.Psychology & Aging,9, 454–463.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Cohen, G., &Faulkner, D. (1988). Life span changes in autobiographical memory. In M. M. Gruneberg, P. E. Morris, & R. N. Sykes (Eds.),Practical aspects of memory: Current research and issues. Vol. 1. Memory in everyday life (pp. 277–282). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  24. Conway, M. A. (1990).Autobiographical memory: An introduction. Milton Keynes, U.K.: Open University Press.Google Scholar
  25. Conway, M. A. (1997). The inventory of experience: Memory and identity. In D. Jodelet, J. Pennebaker, & D. Paez (Eds.),Political events and collective memories (pp. 21–46). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  26. Conway, M. A., &Rubin, D. C. (1993). The structure of autobiographical memory. In A. E. Collins, S. E. Gathercole, M. A. Conway, & P. E. Morris (Eds.),Theories of memory (pp. 103–137). Hove, U.K.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  27. Conway, M. A., Rubin, D. C., Spinnler, H., &Wagenaar, W. A. (Eds.) (1992).Theoretical perspectives on autobiographical memory (pp. 495–499). Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
  28. Craik, F. I. M., &Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,11, 671–684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Crovitz, H. F., &Schiffman, H. (1974). Frequency of episodic memories as a function of their age.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society,4, 517–518.Google Scholar
  30. Dempster, F. N., (1988). The spacing effect: A case study in the failure to apply the results of the psychological research.American Psychologist,43, 627–634.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Erickson, E. H. (1950).Childhood and society. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  32. Fitzgerald, J. M. (1988). Vivid memories and the reminiscence phenomenon: The role of a self narrative.Human Development,31, 261–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Fitzgerald, J. M. (1996). Intersecting meanings of reminiscence in adult development and aging. In D. C. Rubin (Ed.),Remembering our past: Studies in autobiographical memory (pp. 360–383). Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  34. Fitzgerald, J. M., &Lawrence, R. (1984). Autobiographical memory across the life-span.Journal of Gerontology,39, 692–699.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Flanagan, O. J. (1992).Consciousness reconsidered. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  36. Franklin, H. C., &Holding, D. H. (1977). Personal memories at different ages.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,29, 527–532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Fredrickson, B. F., &Carstensen, L. L. (1990). Choosing social partners: How old age and anticipated endings make people more selective.Psychology & Aging,5, 335–347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Fromholt, P., Larsen, P. &Larsen, S. F. (1995). Effects of late-onset depression and recovery on autobiographical memory.Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences,50, 74–81.Google Scholar
  39. Fromholt, P., &Larsen, S. F. (1991). Autobiographical memory in normal aging and primary degenerative dementia (dementia of the Alzheimer type).Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences,46, 85–91.Google Scholar
  40. Fromholt, P., &Larsen, S. F. (1992). Autobiographical memory and life-history narratives in aging and dementia (Alzheimer type). In M. A. Conway, D. C. Rubin, H. Spinnler, & W. A. Wagenaar (Eds.),Theoretical perspectives on autobiographical memory (pp. 413–426). Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
  41. Galton, F. (1879). Psychometric experiments.Brain,2, 149–162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Gardiner, J. M., Craik, F. I. M., &Birtwistle, J. (1972). Retrieval cues and release from proactive inhibition.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,11, 778–783.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Gergen, K. J., &Gergen, M. M. (1983). Narratives of the self. In T. R. Sarbin & K. E. Scheibe (Eds.),Studies in social identity (pp. 254–273). New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
  44. Hale, S., &Myerson, J. (1995). Fifty years older, fifty percent slower? Meta-analytic regression models and semantic context effects.Aging & Cognition,2, 132–145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Henderson, G., Tomlinson, B. E., &Gibson, P. H. (1980). Cell counts in human cerebral cortex in normal adults throughout life using an image analysing computer.Journal of the Neurological Sciences,46, 113–136.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Holbrook, M. B. (1993). Nostalgia and consumption preferences: Some emerging patterns of consumer tastes.Journal of Consumer Research,20, 245–256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Holbrook, M. B., &Schindler, R. M. (1989). Some exploratory findings on the development of musical tastes.Journal of Consumer Research,16, 119–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Howes, J. L., &Katz, A. N. (1988). Assessing remote memory with an improved public events questionnaire.Psychology & Aging,3, 142–150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Howes, J. L., &Katz, A. N. (1992). Remote memory: Recalling autobiographical and public events from across the lifespan.Canadian Journal of Psychology,46, 92–116.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Hunt, R. R., &Einstein, G. O. (1981). Relational and item-specific information in memory.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,20, 497–514.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Hunt, R. R., &McDaniel, M. A. (1993). The enigma of organization and distinctiveness.Journal of Memory & Language,32, 421–445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Hunt, R. R., &Smith, R. E. (1996). Accessing the particular from the general: The power of distinctiveness in the context of organization.Memory & Cognition,24, 217–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Hyland, D. T., &Ackerman, A. M. (1988). Reminiscence and autobiographical memory in the study of the personal past.Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences,43, 35–39.Google Scholar
  54. Jansari, A., &Parkin, A. J. (1996). Things that go bump in your life: Explaining the reminiscence bump in autobiographical memory.Psychology & Aging,11, 85–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Jernigan, T. L., Press, G. A., &Hesselink, J. R. (1990). Methods for measuring brain morphologic features on MRI: Validation and normal aging.Archives of Neurology,47, 27–32.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Kimble, G. A. (1996).Psychology: The hope of a science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  57. Kintsch, W., &van Dijk, T. A. (1978). Toward a model of text comprehension.Psychological Review,85, 363–394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Komura, H., Miyake, A., Chen, C., Tanizawa, O., &Yoshikawa, H. (1992). Relationship of age at menarche and subsequent fertility.European Journal of Obstetrics & Gynecology & Reproductive Biology,44, 201–203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Landauer, T. K., &Bjork, R. A. (1978). Optimum rehearsal pattern and name learning. In M. M. Gruneberg, P. E. Morris, & R. N. Sykes (Eds.)Practical aspects of memory: International Conference on Practical Aspects of Memory (pp. 625–632). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  60. Larsen, S. F. (in press). Memorable books: Recall of reading and its personal context. In M. S. MacNealy & R. Kreuz (Eds.),Empirical approaches to literature and aesthetics (Advances in Discourse Processes, Vol. 52). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  61. Longmore, F. J., Knight, R. G., &Longmore, B. E. (1990). A test of remote memory for use with New Zealand subjects.New Zealand Journal of Psychology,19, 17–23.Google Scholar
  62. Mackavey, W. R., Malley, J. E., &Stewart, A. J. (1991). Remembering autobiographically consequential experiences: Content analysis of psychologists’ accounts of their lives.Psychology & Aging,6, 50–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Mannheim, K. (1952). The problem of generations. In K. Mannheim,Essays on the sociology of knowledge (pp. 276–322). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. (Original work published 1928)Google Scholar
  64. Marslen-Wilson, W. D., &Teuber, H. (1975). Memory for remote events in anterograde amnesia: Recognition of public figures from news photographs.Neuropsychologia,13, 353–364.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Menken, J., Trussell, J., &Larsen, U. (1986). Age and infertility.Science,233, 1389–1394.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Mergler, N. L., &Goldstein, M. D. (1983). Why are there old people: Senescence as biological and cultural preparedness for the transmission of information.Human Development,26, 72–90.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Meudell, P. R., Northen, B., Snowden, J. S., &Neary, D. (1980). Long term memory for famous voices in amnesic and normal subjects.Neuropsychologia,18, 133–139.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Moreland, R. L., &Zajonc, R. B. (1979). Exposure effects may not depend on stimulus recognition.Journal of Personality & Social Psychology,37, 1085–1089.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Noble, C. E., Baker, B. L., &Jones, T. A. (1964). Age and sex parameters in psychomotor learning.Perceptual & Motor Skills,19, 935–945.Google Scholar
  70. Rabbitt, P., &Winthorpe, C. (1988). What do old people remember? The Galton paradigm reconsidered. In M. M. Gruneberg, P. E. Morris, & R.N. Sykes (Eds.),Practical aspects of memory: Current research and issues. Vol. 1. Memory in everyday life (pp. 301–307). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  71. Rasch, G. (1960).Probabilistic models for some intelligence and attainment tests. Copenhagen: Nielsen & Lydiche.Google Scholar
  72. Ribot, T. (1882).Diseases of memory: An essay in the positive psychology (W. H. Smith, Trans.). New York: Appleton.Google Scholar
  73. Ribot, T. (1906).Essay on the creative imagination. (A. H. N. Baron, Trans.). Chicago: Open Court Publishing.Google Scholar
  74. Robinson, J. A. (1976). Sampling autobiographical memory.Cognitive Psychology,8, 578–595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Robinson, J. A. (1992). First experience memories: Contexts and function in personal histories. In M. A. Conway, D. C. Rubin, H. Spinnler, & W. A. Wagenaar (Eds.),Theoretical perspectives on autobiographical memory (pp. 223–239). Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
  76. Robinson, J. A. (1996). Perspective, meaning, and remembering. In D. C. Rubin (Ed.),Remembering our past: Studies in autobiographical memory (pp. 199–217). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  77. Rubin, D. C. (1982). On the retention function for autobiographical memory.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,21, 21–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Rubin, D. C. (1983). Associative asymmetry, availability, and retrieval.Memory & Cognition,11, 83–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Rubin, D. C. (Ed.) (1986).Autobiographical memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  80. Rubin, D. C. (1989). Issues of regularity and control: Confessions of a regularity freak. In L. W. Poon, D. C. Rubin, & B. A. Wilson (Eds.),Everyday cognition in adult and later life (pp. 84–103). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  81. Rubin, D. C. (1995).Memory in oral traditions: The cognitive psychology of epic, ballads, and counting-out rhymes. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  82. Rubin, D. C. (Ed.) (1996).Remembering our past: Studies in autobiographical memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  83. Rubin, D. C., &Schulkind, M. D. (1997a). The distribution of autobiographical memories across the lifespan.Memory & Cognition,25, 859–866.Google Scholar
  84. Rubin, D. C., &Schulkind, M. D. (1997b). The distribution of important and word-cued autobiographical memories in 20-, 35-, and 70-year-old adults.Psychology & Aging,12, 524–535.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Rubin, D. C., &Wenzel, A. E. (1996). One hundred years of forgetting: A quantitative description of retention.Psychological Review,103, 734–760.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Rubin, D. C., Wetzler, S. E., &Nebes, R. D. (1986). Autobiographical memory across the adult lifespan. In D. C. Rubin (Ed.),Autobiographical memory (pp. 202–221). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  87. Salthouse, T. A. (1991). Mediation of adult age differences in cognition by reductions in working memory and speed of processing.Psychological Science,2, 179–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Salthouse, T. A. (1994a). Aging associations: Influence of speed on adult age differences in associative learning.Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory, & Cognition,20, 1486–1503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Salthouse, T. A. (1994b). The nature of the influence of speed on adult age differences in cognition.Developmental Psychology,30, 240–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition.Psychological Review,103, 403–428.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Schuman, H., Belli, R. F., &Bischoping. (1997). The generational basis of historical knowledge. In J.W. Pennebaker, D. Paez, & B. Rime (Eds.),Collective memories of political events (pp. 47–77). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  92. Schuman, H., &Rieger, C. (1992). Collective memory and collective memories. In M. A. Conway, D. C. Rubin, H. Spinnler, & W. A. Wagenaar (Eds.),Theoretical perspectives on autobiographical memory (pp. 323–336). Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
  93. Schuman, H., Rieger, C., &Gaidys, V. (1994). Collective memories in the United States and Lithuania. In N. Schwartz & S. Sudman (Eds.),Autobiographical memory and the validity of retrospective reports (pp. 313–333). New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  94. Schuman, H., &Scott, J. (1989). Generations and collective memories.American Sociological Review,54, 359–381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Sehulster, J. R. (1996). In my era: Evidence for the perception of a special period of the past.Memory,4, 145–158.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Stewart, A. J., &Healy, J. M., Jr. (1989). Linking individual development and social changes.American Psychologist,44, 30–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Storandt, M., Grant, E. A., &Gordon, B. C. (1978). Remote memory as a function of age and sex.Experimental Aging Research,4, 365–375.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Terry, R. D., DeTeresa, R., &Hansen, L. A. (1987). Neocortical cell counts in normal human adult aging.Annals of Neurology,21, 530–539.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Tulving, E., &Pearlstone, Z. (1966). Availability versus accessibility of information in memory for words.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,5, 381–391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. Tulving, E., &Thomson, D. M. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory.Psychological Review,80, 352–373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Tversky, A., &Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability.Cognitive Psychology,5, 207–232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Underwood, B. J. (1957). Interference and forgetting.Psychological Review,64, 49–59.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Warrington, E. K., &Sanders, H. I. (1971). The fate of old memories.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,23, 432–442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Wechsler, D. (1987).Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised: Manual. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  105. Wickens, D. D. (1970). Encoding categories of words: An empirical approach to meaning.Psychological Review,77, 1–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  106. Woodcock, R. W., &Johnson, M. B. (1991).Woodcock-Johnson Psycho-Educational Battery—Revised: Tests of cognitive ability. Allen, TX: DLM Teaching Resources.Google Scholar
  107. Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure.Journal of Personality & Social Psychology Monograph Supplements,9(2, Pt. 2), 1–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  108. Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences.American Psychologist,35, 151–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  109. Zola-Morgan, S., Cohen, N. J., &Squire, L. R. (1983). Recall of remote episodic memory in amnesia.Neuropsychologia,21, 487–500.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 1998

Authors and Affiliations

  • David C. Rubin
    • 1
  • Tamara A. Rahhal
    • 1
  • Leonard W. Poon
    • 2
  1. 1.Experimental PsychologyDuke UniversityDurham
  2. 2.University of GeorgiaAthens

Personalised recommendations