Perception & Psychophysics

, Volume 41, Issue 6, pp 621–634 | Cite as

Dynamic pattern structure in music: Recent theory and research

  • Mari Riess Jones
Article

Abstract

Recent theory and research addressed to dynamic pattern structure in music is reported. Dynamic pattern structure refers to the ways in which rhythm and melody combine; it is summarized here in terms of the constructof joint accent structure. Properties of joint accent structures involving accent couplings and time symmetries are used to address standard psychological issues of pattern similarity and pattern simplicity as they are realized in musical tasks. In particular, experimental work on melody recognition reveals the importance of formalizations concerning dynamic pattern similarities, and other research on reproductions of music-like patterns reveals the potential of a time-based approach to dynamic pattern simplicity.

References

  1. Backus, J. (1969).The acoustical foundations of music. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  2. Benjamin, W. E. (1984). A theory of musical meter.Music Perception,1, 355–413.Google Scholar
  3. Berry, W. (1976).Structural functions in music. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  4. Bharucha, J. J. (1984) Anchoring effects in music. The resolution of dissonance.Cognitive Psychology,16, 485–518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Boltz, M., &Jones, M. R. (1986) Does rule recursion make melodies easier to reproduce? If not, what does?Cognitive Psychology,18, 389–431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Brown, H. (In press). The interplay of set content and temporal context in a functional theory of tonality perception.Music Perception.Google Scholar
  7. Brown, H., &Butler, D. (1981). Diatonic trichord as minimal tonal cue-cells.In Theory Only,5, 39–55.Google Scholar
  8. Browne, R. (1974). Review.Journal of Music Theory,18, 390–409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Butler, D., &Brown, H. (1984). Tonal structure versus function: Studies of the recognition of harmonic motion.Music Perception,2, 6–24.Google Scholar
  10. Clarke, E. F. (1985a). Some aspects of rhythm and expressions in performances of Erik Satie’s “Gnossienne No. 5.”Music Perception,2, 299–328.Google Scholar
  11. Clarke, E. F. (1985b). Structure and expression in rhythmic performance. In P. Howell, I. Cross, & R. West (Eds.),Musical structure and cognition. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  12. Cone, E. (1968).Musical form and musical performance. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  13. Deutsch, D. (1980) The processing of structured and unstructured tonal sequences.Perception & Psychophysics,28, 381–389.Google Scholar
  14. Dowling, W. J. (1978). Scale and contour: Two components of a theory of memory for melodies.Psychological Review,85, 341–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dowling, W. J., &Bartlett, J. C. (1981). The important Interval information in long-term memory for melodies.Psychomusicology,1, 30–49.Google Scholar
  16. Dowling, W. J., &Fujitani, D. S. (1971). Contour, interval and pitch recognition in memory for melodies.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,49, 524–531.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Dyson, M. C., &Watkins, A. J. (1984). A figural approach to the role of melodic contour in melody recognition.Perception & Psychophysics,35, 477–488.Google Scholar
  18. Edworthy, J. (1985). Melodic contour and musical structure. In P. Howell, I. Cross, & R. West (Eds.),Musical structure and cognition. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  19. Essens, P. (1986). Hierarchical organization of temporal patterns.Perception & Psychophysics,40, 69–73.Google Scholar
  20. Fraisse, P. (1978) Time and rhythm perception. In E. C. Carterette & M. P. Friedman (Eds.),Handbook of perception (Vol. 8). New York Academic Press.Google Scholar
  21. Garner, W. R. (1974)The processing of information and structure. Potomac, MD Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  22. Hahn, N., &Jones, M. R. (1981). Invariants in auditory frequency.Scandinavian Journal of Psychology,92, 627–651.Google Scholar
  23. Handel, S. (1984) Using polyrhythms to study rhythm.Music Perception,1, 465–484.Google Scholar
  24. Handel, S., &Oshinsky, J. S. (198l) The meter of syncopated auditory polyrhythms.Perception & Psychophysics,30, 1–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Jones, M. R. (1974) Cognitive representation of serial patterns. In B. Kantowitz (Ed.),Human information processing Tutorials in performance cognition. Potomac, MD: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  26. Jones, M. R. (1976). Time, our lost dimension: Toward a new theory of perception, attention, and memory.Psychological Review,83, 323–355.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Jones, M. R. (1978) Auditory patterns: The perceiving organism. In E. C. Carterette & M. P. Friedman (Eds.),Handbook of perception (Vol. 8). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  28. Jones, M. R. (1981a). Music as a stimulus for psychological motion: Part 1 Some determinants of expectancies.Psychomusicology,1, 34–51.Google Scholar
  29. Jones, M. R. (1981b). Only time can tell: On the topology of mental space and time.Critical Inquiry,7, 557–576.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Jones, M. R. (1981c). A tutorial on some issues and methods in serial pattern research.Perception & Psychophysics,30, 492–504.Google Scholar
  31. Jones, M. R. (1982). Music as a stimulus for psychological motion: Part II. An expectancy model.Psychomusicology,2, 1–13.Google Scholar
  32. Jones, M. R. (1984). The patterning of time and its effects on perceiving.Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,423, 158–167.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Jones, M. R. (1985). Structural organization of events in time: A review. In J. A. Michon & J. L. Jackson (Eds.),Time, mind, and behavior. Heidelberg Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  34. Jones, M. R. (1987). Perspectives on musical time. In A. Gabnelsson (Ed.),Action and perception in rhythm and music. Stockholm The Royal Swedish Academy of Music Publications.Google Scholar
  35. Jones, M. R., & Boltz, M (1987).Dynamic attending and responses to time. Manuscript submitted for publicationGoogle Scholar
  36. Jones, M. R., Boltz, M., &Kidd, G. (1982). Controlled attending as a function of melodic and temporal context.Perception & Psychophysics,32, 211–218.Google Scholar
  37. Jones, M. R., &Hahn, J. (1986). Invariants sound. In V. McCabe & G. Balzano (Eds.),Event cognition: An ecological perspective. Hillsdale, NJ. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  38. Jones, M. R., Kidd, G., &Wetzel, R. (1981) Evidence for rhythmic attention.Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception & Performance,7, 1059–1073.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Jones, M. R., Summerell, ., &Marshburn, E. (1987) Recognizing melodies A dynamic interpretation.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,39, 89–121.Google Scholar
  40. Kallman, H. J., &Massaro, D. W. (1979) Tone schroma is functional In melody recognition.Perception & Psychophysics,26, 32–36.Google Scholar
  41. Krumhansl, C. L., &Kessler, E. J. (1982) Tracing the dynamic changes in perceived tonal organization in a spatial representation of musical keys.Psychological Review,89, 334–368.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Lerdahl, F., &Jackendoff.R. (1983).A generative theory of tonal music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  43. Martin, J. (1972). Rhythmic (hierarchical) vs. serial structure in speech and other behavior.Psychological Review,79, 487–509.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Povel, D.-J., &Essens, P. (1985). Perception of temporal patterns.Music Perception,2, 411–440.Google Scholar
  45. Povel, D.-J., &Okkerman, H. (1981) Accents in equitone sequences.Perception & Psychophysics,30, 565–572.Google Scholar
  46. Shaffer, L. H. (1982) Rhythm and timing in skill.Psychological Review,89, 109–122.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Simon, H. (1972) Complexity and the representation of patterned sequences of symbols.Psychological Review,79, 369–382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Sloboda, J. A. (1985).The musical mind. Fair Lawn, NJ: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  49. Thomassen, J. M. (1982) Melodic accent Experiments and a tentative model.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,71, 1596–1605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Todd, N. (1985). A model of expressive timing in tonal music.Music Perception,3, 33–58.Google Scholar
  51. Watkins, A. J. (1985) Scale, key, and contour in the discrimination of tuned and mistuned approximations to melody.Perception & Psychophysics,37, 275–285.Google Scholar
  52. Welker, R. L. (1982). Abstraction of themes from melodic variations.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,8, 435–447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Woodrow, H. (1951) Time perception. In S. S. Stevens (Ed.),Handbook of experimental psychology. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  54. Yeston, M. (1976).The stratification of musical rhythm. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 1987

Authors and Affiliations

  • Mari Riess Jones
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.The Netherlands Institute for Advanced StudyWassenaarThe Netherlands
  2. 2.The Ohio State UniversityColumbus

Personalised recommendations