Advertisement

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

, Volume 5, Issue 4, pp 694–697 | Cite as

Imitation in Japanese quail: The role of reinforcement of demonstrator responding

  • Chana K. Akins
  • Thomas R. ZentallEmail author
Brief Reports

Abstract

Imitative learning has been difficult to demonstrate in animals, partly because techniques have not adequately ruled out alternative accounts based on motivational and perceptual mechanisms (Zentall, 1996). Recently, it has been proposed that differences in the effects of observation of two very different responsetopographies would rule out such artifactual, alternative accounts (Akins & Zentall, 1996). In the present research, we confirmed that strong evidence for imitation can be found in Japanese quail, and that such imitation requires the imitator’s observation ofreinforced responding by the demonstrator. Thus, under the present conditions, it appears that an observer will imitate a demonstrated responseonly if it also observes the appetitive consequences of that response.

Keywords

Japanese Quail Imitative Behavior Response Topography Imitative Learning Stimulus Enhancement 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Akins, C. K., &Zentall, T. R. (1996). Imitative learning in male Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) using the two-action method.Journal of Comparative Psychology,110, 316–320.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Bandura, A. (1962). Social learning though imitation. In M. R. Jones (Ed.),Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 211–269). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
  3. Bandura, A., Ross, D., &Ross, S. A. (1963). Vicarious reinforcement and imitation.Journal of Abnormal & Social Psychology,67, 601–607.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bandura, A., &Walters, R. H. (1963).Social learning and personality development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
  5. Berger, S. M. (1961). Incidental learning through vicarious reinforcement.Psychological Reports,9, 477–491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Darby, C. L., &Riopelle, A. J. (1959). Observational learning in the rhesus monkey.Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology,54, 94–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Dawson, B. V., &Foss, B. M. (1965). Observational learning in budgerigars.Animal Behaviour,13, 470–474.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Galef, B. G., Manzig, L. A., &Field, R. M. (1986). Imitation learning in budgerigars: Dawson and Foss (1965) revisited.Behavioural Processes,13, 191–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Gewirtz, J. L. (1969). Mechanisms of social learning: Some roles of stimulation and behavior in early human development. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.),Handbook of socialization theory and research (pp. 57–211). Chicago: Rand-McNally.Google Scholar
  10. Groesbeck, R. W., &Duerfeldt, P. H. (1971). Some relevant variables in observational learning of the rat.Psychonomic Science,22, 41–43.Google Scholar
  11. Heyes, C. M. (1996). Introduction: Identifying and defining imitation. In C. M. Heyes & B. G. Galef (Eds.),Social learning in animals: The roots of culture (pp. 211–220). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Heyes, C. M., &Dawson, G. R. (1990). A demonstration of observational learning using a bidirectional control.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,42B, 59–71.Google Scholar
  13. Heyes, C. M., Jaldow, E., &Dawson, G. R. (1994). Imitation in rats: Conditions of occurrence in a bidirectional control procedure.Learning & Motivation,25, 276–287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Kanfer, F. H. (1965). Vicarious human reinforcement: A glimpse into the black box. In L. Kramer (Ed.),Research in behavior modification (pp. 244–267). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
  15. Kanfer, F. H., &Marston, A. R. (1963). Human reinforcement: Vicarious and direct.Journal of Experimental Psychology,65, 292–296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Lewis, D. J., &Duncan, C. P. (1958). Vicarious experience and partial reinforcement.Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology,57, 321–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. McBrearty, J. F., Marston, A. R., &Kanfer, F. H. (1961). Conditioning a verbal operant in a group setting: Direct vs. vicarious reinforcement.American Psychologist,16, 425.Google Scholar
  18. Meltzoff, A. N. (1996). The human infant as imitative generalist: A 20-year progress report on infant imitation with implications for comparative psychology. In C. M. Heyes & B. G. Galef (Eds.),Social learning in animals: The roots of culture (pp. 347–370). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Meltzoff, A. N., &Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates.Science,198, 75–78.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Miller, N. E., &Dollard, J. (1941).Social learning and imitation. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  21. Piaget, J. (1945).Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  22. Warden, C. J., Fjeld, H. A., &Koch, A. M. (1940). Imitative behavior in the cebus and rhesus monkeys.Journal of Genetic Psychology,56, 311–322.Google Scholar
  23. Whiten, A. (1991).Natural theories of mind: Evolution, development and simulation of everyday mindreading. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  24. Zentall, T. R. (1996). An analysis of imitative learning in animals. In C. M. Heyes & B. G. Galef (Eds.),Social learning in animals: The roots of culture (pp. 221–243). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Zentall, T. R., Sutton, J. E., &Sherburne, L. M. (1996). True imitative learning in pigeons.Psychological Science,7, 343–346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 1998

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of KentuckyLexington

Personalised recommendations