Skip to main content

Advertisement

SpringerLink
  • Log in
  1. Home
  2. Animal Learning & Behavior
  3. Article
The principle of aggregation in psychobiological correlational research: An example from the open-field test
Download PDF
Your article has downloaded

Similar articles being viewed by others

Slider with three articles shown per slide. Use the Previous and Next buttons to navigate the slides or the slide controller buttons at the end to navigate through each slide.

Inter-individual differences in laboratory rats as revealed by three behavioural tasks

07 June 2022

Veronika Rudolfová, Tomáš Petrásek, … Tereza Nekovářová

Thinking thrice about sum scores, and then some more about measurement and analysis

25 April 2022

Keith F. Widaman & William Revelle

Multifactor Modeling and Analysis of Repeated Observations of Biological Objects

01 April 2021

A. G. Belov, A. E. Polienko & O. A. Belova

The paired A–Not A design within signal detection theory: Description, differentiation, power analysis and application

07 February 2022

Nina Düvel & Reinhard Kopiez

The multivariate analysis of variance as a powerful approach for circular data

27 April 2022

Lukas Landler, Graeme D. Ruxton & E. Pascal Malkemper

Quantifying free behaviour in an open field using k-motif approach

27 December 2019

Marein Könings, Mark Blokpoel, … Natalia Z. Bielczyk

How to Design Experiments in Animal Behaviour

01 August 2018

Raghavendra Gadagkar

Permutation tests are robust and powerful at 0.5% and 5% significance levels

28 May 2021

Kimihiro Noguchi, Frank Konietschke, … Markus Pauly

Mixed effects modeling of Morris water maze data revisited: Bayesian censored regression

22 February 2021

Michael E. Young & Michael R. Hoane

Download PDF
  • Published: December 1985

The principle of aggregation in psychobiological correlational research: An example from the open-field test

  • Klaus-Peter Ossenkopp1 &
  • Dwight S. Mazmanian1 

Animal Learning & Behavior volume 13, pages 339–344 (1985)Cite this article

  • 2737 Accesses

  • 36 Citations

  • Metrics details

Abstract

The principle of aggregation states that the sum of a set of multiple measurements is a more stable and representative estimator than any single measurement. This greater representation occurs because there is inevitably some error associated with measurement. By combining numerous exemplars, such errors of measurement are averaged out, leaving a clearer view of underlying relationships. The present study explored the effect of score aggregation over various time periods on correlations among a number of reliable measures frequently used in open-field testing. Twenty-six male rats were given four open-field tests (4 min in duration) at 48-h intervals. Ambulation, rearing, and defecation responses were measured on a minute-by-minute basis in the open-field tests. Correlation matrices were calculated among the three measures for unaggregated scores (1-min totals) and for scores aggregated over daily tests, and mean correlation coefficients were computed for all three pairwise comparisons of the three response variables. These mean correlations were then compared to those obtained when the open-field measures were aggregated over all 4 test days. The results showed that aggregation produced substantial increases in correlation-coefficient magnitude. The correlation between ambulation and rearing increased from a mean of .39 to a value of .81. Similar increases were observed when defecation scores were correlated with ambulation (−.17 to −.59) and rearing (−.16 to −.49). Thus aggregation is an important factor to be considered in the design of psychobiological correlational studies.

Download to read the full article text

Working on a manuscript?

Avoid the most common mistakes and prepare your manuscript for journal editors.

Learn more

References

  • Anderson, E. E. (1938). The interrelationship of drives in the male albino rat: III. Interrelationships among measures of emotional, sexual, and exploratory behavior.Journal of Genetic Psychology,53, 335–352.

    Google Scholar 

  • Archer, J. (1973). Tests for emotionality in rats and mice: A review.Animal Behaviour,21, 205–235.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, S. (1979). The stability of behavior: I. On predicting most of the people much of the time.Journal of Personality & Social Psychology,37, 1097–1126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, S. (1980). The stability of behavior: II. Implications for psychological research.American Psychologist,35, 790–806.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gulliksen, H. (1950).Theory of mental tests. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hall, C. S. (1934). Emotional behavior in the rat: Defecation and urination as measures of individual differences in emotionality.Journal of Comparative Psychology,18, 385–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, C. S. (1936). Emotional behavior in the rat: III. The relationship between emotionality and ambulatory activity.Journal of Comparative Psychology,22, 345–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, C. S. &Ballechey, E. L. (1932). A study of the rat’s behavior in a field: A contribution to method in comparative psychology.University of California Publication in Psychology,6, 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harshman, R. A. (1970). Foundations of the parafac procedure: Models and conditions for an “explanatory” multi-model factor analysis.UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics (No. 16), 1–86.

  • Holland, H. C., &Gupta, B. D. (1966). Some correlated measures of activity and reactivity in two strains of rats selectively bred for differences in the acquisition of a conditioned avoidance response.Animal Behaviour,14, 574–580.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ivinskis, A. (1968). The reliability of behavioural measures obtained in the open-field.Australian Journal of Psychology,20, 173–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, R. M., &Novick, M. R. (1968).Statistical theories of mental test scores. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ossenkopp, K.-P., &Mazmanian, D. S. (1985a). The measurement and integration of behavioral variables: Aggregation and complexity as important issues.Neurobehavioral Toxicology & Teratology,7, 95–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ossenkopp, K.-P., &Mazmanian, D. S. (1985b). Some behavioral factors related to the effects of cold-restraint stress in rats: A factor analytic-multiple regression approach.Physiology & Behavior,34, 935–941.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ossenkopp, K.-P., Sorensen, L., & Mazmanian, D. S. (1986).Factor analysis of open-field behavior in rats: Using the three-way PARAFAC model for longitudinal data. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Pare, W. P. (1964). Relationship of various behaviors in the open-field test of emotionality.Psychological Reports,14, 19–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ray, O., &Hockhauser, S. (1969). Growth hormone and environmental complexity effects on behavior in the rat.Developmental Psychology,1, 311–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rushton, J. P., Brainerd, C. J., &Pressley, M. (1983). Behavioral development and construct validity: The principle of aggregation.Psychological Bulletin,94, 18–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spearman, C. (1910). Correlation calculated from faulty data.British Journal of Psychology,3, 271–295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tachibana, T. (1980). The open-field test: An approach from multivariate analysis.Animal Learning & Behavior,8, 465–467.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tachibana, T. (1982). Open-field test for rats: Correlational analysis.Psychological Reports,50, 899–910.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walsh, R. N., &Cummins, R. A. (1976). The open-field test: A critical review.Psychological Bulletin,83, 482–504.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whimbey, A. E., &Denenberg, V. H. (1967). Two independent behavioral dimensions in open-field performance.Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology,63, 500–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Will, B. E. (1977). Neurochemical correlates of individual differences in animal learning capacity.Behavioral Biology,19, 143–171.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

  1. Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario, N6A 5C2, London, Ontario, Canada

    Klaus-Peter Ossenkopp & Dwight S. Mazmanian

Authors
  1. Klaus-Peter Ossenkopp
    View author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

  2. Dwight S. Mazmanian
    View author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

Additional information

This study was supported by a grant from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (U0151) to the first author.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Ossenkopp, KP., Mazmanian, D.S. The principle of aggregation in psychobiological correlational research: An example from the open-field test. Animal Learning & Behavior 13, 339–344 (1985). https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03208007

Download citation

  • Received: 10 December 1984

  • Accepted: 12 December 1984

  • Issue Date: December 1985

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03208007

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

Keywords

  • Defecation Measure
  • Fecal Boli
  • Defecation Score
  • Ambulation Score
  • Score Aggregation
Download PDF

Working on a manuscript?

Avoid the most common mistakes and prepare your manuscript for journal editors.

Learn more

Advertisement

Over 10 million scientific documents at your fingertips

Switch Edition
  • Academic Edition
  • Corporate Edition
  • Home
  • Impressum
  • Legal information
  • Privacy statement
  • California Privacy Statement
  • How we use cookies
  • Manage cookies/Do not sell my data
  • Accessibility
  • FAQ
  • Contact us
  • Affiliate program

Not logged in - 3.238.250.73

Not affiliated

Springer Nature

© 2023 Springer Nature Switzerland AG. Part of Springer Nature.