Adams, R. M. (1995). Momentum in the performance of professional tournament pocket billiards players.International Journal of Sport Psychology,26, 580–587.
Google Scholar
Ayton, P. (1998, September 19). Fallacy football.New Scientist,159, 52.
Google Scholar
Ayton, P., Hunt, A. J., &Wright, G. (1989). Psychological conceptions of randomness.Journal of Behavioral Decision Making,2, 221–238.
Article
Google Scholar
Ayton, P., Hunt, A. J., &Wright, G. (1991). Randomness and reality.Journal of Behavioral Decision Making,4, 222–226.
Article
Google Scholar
Bar-Hillel, M., &Wagenaar, W.-A. (1991). The perception of randomness.Advances in Applied Mathematics,12, 428–454.
Article
Google Scholar
Boynton D. M. (2003). Superstitious responding and frequency matching in the positive bias and gambler’s fallacy effects.Organizational Behavior & Human Decision Processes,91, 119–127.
Article
Google Scholar
Budescu, D. V. (1987). A Markov model for generation of random binary sequences.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,13, 25–39.
Article
Google Scholar
Burns, B. D. (2003). When it is adaptive to follow streaks: Variability and stocks. In R. Alterman & D. Kirsh (Eds.),Proceedings of the Twenty-Fifth Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Burns, B. D. (2004). Heuristics as beliefs and as behaviors: The adaptiveness of the “hot hand”.Cognitive Psychology,48, 295–331.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Burns, B. D., &Corpus, B. (2004). Randomness and inductions from streaks: “Gambler’s fallacy” versus “hot hand”.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,11, 179–184.
Google Scholar
Burrows, B. L., &Talbot, R. F. (1985). Boycott, Botham and the exponential distribution.Teaching Statistics,7, 42–47.
Article
Google Scholar
Camerer, C. F. (1989). Does the basketball market believe in the “hot hand”.American Economic Review,79, 1257–1261.
Google Scholar
Clark, R. E., Manns, J. R., &Squire, L. R. (2001). Trace and delay eyeblink conditioning: Contrasting phenomena of declarative and nondeclarative memory.Psychological Science,12, 304–308.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Clotfelter, C. T., &Cook, P. J. (1993). The “gambler’s fallacy” in lottery play.Management Science,39, 1521–1525.
Article
Google Scholar
Derks, P. L. (1962). The generality of the “conditioning axiom” in human binary prediction.Journal of Experimental Psychology,63, 538–545.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Derks, P. L. (1963). Effect of run length on the gambler’s fallacy.Journal of Experimental Psychology,65, 213–214.
Article
Google Scholar
Dorsey-Palmateer, R., &Smith, G. (2004). Bowlers’ hot hands.The American Statistician,58, 38–45.
Article
Google Scholar
Edwards, W. (1961). Probability learning in 1,000 trials.Journal of Experimental Psychology,62, 385–394.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Estes, W. K. (1964). Probability learning. In A. W. Melton (Ed.),Categories of human learning (pp. 89–128). New York: Academic Press.
Google Scholar
Falk, R. (1975). Perception of randomness. Doctoral dissertation (in Hebrew with English abstract). Jerusalem: Hebrew University.
Falk, R. (1981). The perception of randomness. InProceedings of the Fifth International Conference for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (pp. 222–229). Grenoble, France.
Falk, R., &Konold, C. (1994). Random means hard to digest.Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics,16, 2–12.
Google Scholar
Falk, R., &Konold, C. (1997). Making sense of randomness: Implicit encoding as a basis for judgment.Psychological Review,104, 301–318.
Article
Google Scholar
Gilden, D. L., &Wilson, S. G. (1995). On the nature of streaks in signaldetection.Cognitive Psychology,28, 17–64.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Gilden, D. L., &Wilson, S. G. (1996). Streaks in skilled performance.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,2, 260–265.
Google Scholar
Gilovich, T. (1991). How we know what isn’t so: The fallibility of human reason in everyday life. New York: Free Press.
Google Scholar
Gilovich, T., Vallone, R., &Tversky, A. (1985). The hot hand in basketball: On the misperception of random sequences.Cognitive Psychology,17, 295–314.
Article
Google Scholar
Hooke, R. (1989). Basketball, baseball, and the null hypothesis.Chance: New Directions for Statistics & Computing,2, 35–37.
Google Scholar
Jarvik, M. E. (1951). Probability learning and a negative recency effect in the serial anticipation of alternative symbols.Journal of Experimental Psychology,41, 291–297.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., &Frederick, S. (2002). Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.),Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment (pp. 49–81). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., &Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective probability: A judgment of representativeness.Cognitive Psychology,3, 430–454.
Article
Google Scholar
Laplace, P. S. de (1951). A philosophical essay on probabilities. New York: Dover. (Original work published 1796)
Google Scholar
Larkey, P. D., Smith, R. A., &Kadane, J. B. (1989). It’s okay to believe in the “hot hand”.Chance: New Directions for Statistics and Computing,2, 22–30.
Google Scholar
Lee, W. (1971). Decision theory and human behavior. New York: Wiley.
Google Scholar
Lopes, L. L. (1982). Doing the impossible: A note on induction and the experience of randomness.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,8, 626–636.
Article
Google Scholar
Lopes, L. L., &Oden, G. (1987). Distinguishing between random and nonrandom events.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,13, 392–400.
Article
Google Scholar
Neuringer, A. (1989). Can people behave randomly? The role of feedback.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,115, 62–75.
Article
Google Scholar
Perruchet, P. (1985). A pitfall for the expectancy theory of human eyelid conditioning.Pavlovian Journal of Biological Sciences,20, 163–170.
Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York: Norton.
Google Scholar
Smith, G. (2003). Horseshoe pitchers’ hot hands.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,10, 753–758.
Article
Google Scholar
Sundali, J., Croson, R., & Gold, E. (2000, May) The gambler’s fallacy versus the hot hand: Empirical data from casinos.Seventh Conference on Behavioral Decision Research in Management. Tucson, Arizona.
Treisman, M., &Faulkner, A. (1990). Generation of random sequences by human subjects: Cognitive operations or psychophysical process?Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,116, 337–355.
Article
Google Scholar
Tversky, A., &Gilovich, T. (1989a). The cold facts about the “hot hand” in basketball.Chance: New Directions for Statistics & Computing,2, 16–21.
Google Scholar
Tversky, A., &Gilovich, T. (1989b). The “hot hand”: Statistical reality or cognitive illusion?Chance: New Directions for Statistics & Computing,2, 31–34.
Google Scholar
Wagenaar, W.-A. (1988).Paradoxes of gambling behavior. Hove, U.K.: Erlbaum.
Google Scholar
Wagenaar, W.-A., &Keren, G. (1988). Chance and luck are not the same.Journal of Behavioral Decision Making,1, 65–75.
Article
Google Scholar
Wardrop, R. L. (1995). Simpson’s paradox and the hot hand in basketball.The American Statistician,49, 24–28.
Article
Google Scholar
Wardrop, R. L. (1998). Basketball. In J. Bennett (Ed.),Statistics in sport (pp. 65–82). London: Arnold.
Google Scholar
Witte, R. S. (1964). Long-term effects of patterned reward schedules.Journal of Experimental Psychology,68, 588–594.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar