Advertisement

Perception & Psychophysics

, Volume 62, Issue 2, pp 253–265 | Cite as

The perception of primary and secondary stress in English

  • Sven L. MattysEmail author
Article

Abstract

Most models of word recognition concerned with prosody are based on a distinction between strong syllables (containing a full vowel) and weak syllables (containing a schwa). In these models, the possibility that listeners take advantage of finer grained prosodie distinctions, such as primary versus secondary stress, is usually rejected on the grounds that these two categories are not discriminable from each other without lexical information or normalization of the speaker’s voice. In the present experiment, subjects were presented with word fragments that differed only by their degree of stress— namely, primary or secondary stress (e.g., /prasi/ vs. /prasi/). The task was to guess the origin of the fragment (e.g., “prosecutor” vs. “prosecution”). The results showed that guessing performance significantly exceeds the chance level, which indicates that making fine stress distinctions is possible without lexical information and with minimal speech normalization. This finding is discussed in the framework of prosody-based word recognition theories.

Keywords

Word Recognition Speech Perception Lexical Access Acoustical Society ofAmerica Word Boundary 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Beckman, M. E. (1986).Stress and non-stress accent. Dordrecht: Fions.Google Scholar
  2. Brandt, J. F., Ruder, K. P., &Shipp, I., Jr. (1969). Vocal loudness and effort in continuous speech.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,46, 1543–1548.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Burzio, L. (1994).Principles of English stress. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Carlson, R., Grandstrom, B., Lindblom, B., &Rapp, K. (1973). Some timing and fundamental frequency characteristics of Swedish sentences: Data, rules, and perceptual evaluation.Speech Transmission Laboratory Quarterly Progress Status Report,4, 11–19.Google Scholar
  5. Cole, R. A., &Jakimik, J. (1980a). How are syllables used to recognize words?Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,67, 965–970.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Cole, R. A., &Jakimik, J. (1980b). A model of speech perception. In R. Cole (Ed.),Perception and production of fluent speech (pp. 133–163). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  7. Cutler, A. (1986). Forbear is an homophone: Lexical prosody does not constrain lexical access,language & Speech,29, 201–220.Google Scholar
  8. Cutler, A., &Butterfield, S. (1992). Rhythmic cues to speech segmentation: Evidence from juncture misperception.Journal of Memory & Language,31, 218–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Cutler, A., &Carter, D. M. (1987). The predominance of strong initial syllables in the English vocabulary.Computer Speech & Language,2, 133–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cutler, A., &Norris, D. G. (1988). The role of strong syllables in segmentation for lexical access.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,14, 113–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Echols, C. H., Crowhurst, M. J., &Childers, J. B. (1997). Perception of rhythmic units in speech by infants and adults.Journal of Memory & Language,36, 202–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Fear, B. D., Cutler, A., &Butterfield, S. (1995). The strong/weak syllable distinction in English.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,97, 1893–1904.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Fry, D. B. (1955). Duration and intensity as physical correlates of linguistic stress.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,27, 765–768.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Fry, D. B. (1958). Experiments in the perception of stress.Language & Speech,1, 126–152.Google Scholar
  15. Grosjean, F., &Gee, J. P. (1987). Prosodie structure and spoken word recognition.Cognition,25, 135–155.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Hayes, B. (1995).Metrical stress theory. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
  17. Hermes, D. J., &Rump, H. H. (1994). Perception of prominence in speech intonation induced by rising and falling pitch movements.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,96, 83–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hermes, D. J., &Van Gestel, J. C. (1991). The frequency scale of speech intonation.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,90, 97–102.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Jusczyk, P. W., Cutler, A., &Redanz, N. (1993). Preference for the predominant stress patterns of English words.Child Development,64, 675–687.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Klatt, D. H. (1980). Speech perception: A model of acoustic-phonetic analysis and lexical access. In R. A. Cole (Ed.),Perception and production of fluent speech (pp. 243–288). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  21. Kucera, H., &Francis, W. (1967).Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press.Google Scholar
  22. Lehiste, I. (1970).Suprasegmentals. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  23. Liberman, A. M., &Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1978). Phonetic perception. In R. Held, H. W. Leibowicz, & H. L. Teuber (Eds.),Handbook of sensory physiology (pp. 143–178). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  24. Liberman, M. Y., &Prince, A. (1977). On stress and linguistic rhythm.Linguistic Inquiry,8, 249–336.Google Scholar
  25. Liberman, M. Y, &Streeter, L. A. (1978). Use of nonsense-syllable mimicry in the study of prosodie phenomena.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,63, 231 -233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Lieberman, P. (1960). Some acoustic correlates of word stress in American English.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,32, 451 -454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Lieberman, P. (1965). On the acoustic basis of perception of stress by linguists.Word,21, 40–54.Google Scholar
  28. Luce, P. A. (1986). A computational analysis of uniqueness points in auditory word recognition.Perception & Psychophysics,39, 155–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Luce, P. A., &Cluff, M. S. (1998). Delayed commitment in spoken word recognition: Evidence from cross-modal priming.Perception & Psychophysics,60, 484–490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Marcus, S. M. (1984). Recognizing speech: On the mapping from sound to word. In H. Bouma & D. G. Bouwhuis (Eds),Attention and performance X: Control of language processes (pp. 151–163). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  31. Mattys, S. L., Jusczyk, P. W., Luce, P. A., &Morgan, J. L. (1999). Phonotactic and prosodie effects on word segmentation in infants.Cognitive Psychology,38, 465–494.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Mattys, S. L., &Samuel, A. G. (1997). How lexical stress affects speech segmentation and interactivity: Evidence from the migration paradigm.Journal of Memory & Language,36, 87–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Mattys, S. L., & Samuel, A. G. (in press). Implications of stress pattern differences in spoken word recognition.Journal of Memory & Language.Google Scholar
  34. McQueen, J. M., Cutler, A., Briscoe, T., &Norris, D. (1995). Models of continuous speech recognition and the contents of the vocabulary.Language & Cognitive Processes,10, 309–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Meltzer, R. H., Martin, J. G., Mills, C B., Imhoff, D. L., &Zohar, D. (1976). Anticipatory coarticulation and reaction time to phoneme targets in spontaneous speech.Phonetica,37, 159–168.Google Scholar
  36. Mens, L. H., &Povel, D.-J. (1986). Evidence against a predictive role for rhythm in speech perception.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,38A, 177–192.Google Scholar
  37. Morgan, J. L. (1996). A rhythmic bias in preverbal speech segmentation.Journal of Memory & Language,35, 666–688.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Morton, J., &Jassem, W. (1965). Acoustic correlates of stress.Language & Speech,8, 148–158.Google Scholar
  39. Nakatani, L. H., &Schaffer, J. A. (1978). Hearing ‘words’ without words: Prosodie cues for word perception.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,63, 234–245.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Nooteboom, S. G., Brokx, J. P. L., &De Rooij, J. J. (1978). Contribution of prosody to speech perception. In W. J. M. Levelt & G. B. Flores d’Arcais (Eds.),Studies in the perception of language (pp. 75–107). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  41. Norris, D., McQueen, J. M., &Cutler, A. (1995). Competition and segmentation in spoken word recognition.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,21, 1209–1228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Pierrehumbert, J. (1979). The perception of fundamental frequency declination.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,66, 363–369.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Pitt, M. A., &Samuel, A. G. (1990). The use of rhythm in attending to speech.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,16, 564–573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Rietveld, A. C. M., &Koopmans-Van Beinum, F. J. (1987). Vowel reduction and stress.Speech Communication,6, 217–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Selkirk, E. O. (1984).Phonology and syntax: The relation between sound and structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  46. Shields, J. L., McHugh, A., &Martin, J. G. (1974). Reaction time to phoneme targets as a function of rhythmic cues in continuous speech.Journal of Experimental Psychology,102, 250–255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Sluijter, A. M. C. &Van Heuven, V. J. (1996). Spectral balance as an acoustic correlate of linguistic stress.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,100, 2471–2485.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Sluijter, A. M. C, Van Heuven, V. J., &Pacilly, J. J. A. (1997). Spectral balance as a cue in the perception of linguistic stress.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,101, 503–513.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Strange, W., &Gottfried, T. (1980). Task variables in the study of vowel perception.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,68, 1622–1625.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Strange, W., Verbrugge, R., Shankweiler, D., &Edman, T. (1976). Consonant environment specifies vowel identity.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,60, 213–224.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Van Bergem, D. R., Pols, L. C. W., &Koopmans-Van Beinum, F. J. (1988). Perceptual normalization of the vowels of a man and a child in various contexts.Speech Communication,7, 1–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Van Heuven, V. J., &Menert, L. (1996). Why stress position bias?Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,100, 2439–2451.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Vroomen, J., & De Gelder, B. (1997).Trochaic rhythm in speech segmentation. Paper presented at the 38th Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Philadelphia.Google Scholar
  54. Vroomen, J., Tuomainen, J., &De Gelder, B. (1998). The roles of word stress and vowel harmony in speech segmentation.Journal of Memory & Language,38, 133–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Waibel, A. (1986). Suprasegmentals in very large vocabulary word recognition speech perceptions. In E. C. Schwab & H. C. Nusbaum (Eds.),Pattern recognition by humans and machines (pp. 159–186). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2000

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.State University of New YorkStony Brook

Personalised recommendations