Memory & Cognition

, Volume 21, Issue 2, pp 146–155 | Cite as

The recency effect: Implicit learning with explicit retrieval?

  • Alan D. Baddeley
  • Graham Hitch
Article

Abstract

The recency effect in free recall features prominently in 1960s' theorizing about short-term memory, but has since been largely ignored. We argue that this stems from a preoccupation with the role of recency in the concept of primary memory and the neglect of its role in a broader working-memory framework. It is suggested that the recency effect reflects the application of an explicit retrieval strategy to the residue of implicit learning within a range of cognitive systems. When retrieved implicitly, the same residue is assumed to form the basis of priming effects. The various criteria for implicit learning described by Tulving and Schacter (1990) are successfully applied to the recency effect, and a retrieval process is outlined that can account for both long- and short-term recency effects. It is suggested that a framework combining recency, priming, and implicit learning provides a basis for understanding one of the most important features of cognition and memory, namely, that of maintaining orientation in time and place.

References

  1. Atkinson, R. C., &Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In K. W. Spence (Ed.),The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 2, pp. 89–195). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  2. Baddeley, A. D. (1968). Prior recall of newly learned items and the recency effect in free recall.Canadian Journal of Psychology,22, 157–163.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Baddeley, A. D. (1970). Estimating the short-term component in free recall.British Journal of Psychology,61, 13–15.Google Scholar
  4. Baddeley, A. D. (1981). The cognitive psychology of everyday life.British Journal of Psychology,72, 257–269.Google Scholar
  5. Baddeley, A. D. (1982). Amnesia: A minimal model and an interpretation. In L. S. Cermak (Ed.),Human memory and amnesia (pp. 305–336). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  6. Baddeley, A. D. (1986).Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Baddeley, A. D. (1988). But what the hell is it for? In M. M. Gruneberg, P. E. Morris, & R. N. Sykes (Eds.),Practical aspects of memory: Current research and issues: Vol. 1. Memory in everyday life (pp. 3–18). Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
  8. Baddeley, A. D. (1990).Human memory: Theory and practice. London: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  9. Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Working memory.Science,255, 556–559.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Baddeley, A. D., &Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. A. Bower (Ed.),The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  11. Baddeley, A. D., &Hitch, G. (1977). Recency re-examined. In S. Dornic (Ed.),Attention and performance (Vol. 6, pp. 647–667). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  12. Baddeley, A. D., &Warrington, E. K. (1970). Amnesia and the distinction between long- and short-term memory.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,9, 176–189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Baddeley, A. D., &Wilson, B. A. (1988), Frontal amnesia and the dysexecutive syndrome.Brain & Cognition,7, 212–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Bjork, R. A., &Whitten, W. B. (1974). Recency-sensitive retrieval processes.Cognitive Psychology,6, 173–189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Brooks, D. N. (1975). Long- and short-term memory in head injured patients.Cortex,11, 329–340.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Brown, N. R., Shevell, S. K., &Rips, L. J. (1986). Public memories and their personal context. In D. C. Rubin (Ed.),Autobiographical memory (pp. 137–158). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  17. Burgess, N., Shapiro, J. L., &Moore, M. A. (1991). Neural network models of list learning.Network,2, 399–422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Cohen, R. L., &Sandberg, T. (1977). Relation between intelligence and short-term memory.Cognitive Psychology,9, 534–554.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Craik, F. I. M. (1968). Two components in free recall.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,7, 996–1004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Craik, F. I. M. (1970). The fate of primary memory items in free recall.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,9, 143–148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Craik, F. I. M., &Levy, B. A. (1970). Semantic and acoustic information in primary memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology,86, 77–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Craik, F. I. M., &Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,11, 671–684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Crowder, R. G. (1976).Principles of learning and memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  24. Dalezman, J. J. (1976). Effects of output order on immediate, delayed and final recall performance.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory,2, 597–608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Darley, C. F, Tinklenberg, J. R., Roth, W. T., Hollister, L. E., &Atkinson, R. C. (1973). Influence of marihuana on storage and retrieval processes in memory.Memory & Cognition,1, 196–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Glanzer, M. (1972). Storage mechanisms in recall. In G. H. Bower (Ed.),The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 5, pp. 129–193). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  27. Glanzer, M., &Cunitz, A. R. (1966). Two storage mechanisms in free recall.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,5, 351–360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Glanzer, M., &Koppenaal, L. (1977). The effect of encoding tasks on free recall: Stages and levels.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,16, 21–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Glenberg, A. M, Bradley, M. M, Kraus, T. A., &Renzaglia, G. J. (1983). Studies of the long-term recency effect: Support for a contextually guided retrieval hypothesis.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,9, 231–255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Glenberg, A. M, Bradley, M. M, Stevenson, J. A, Kraus, T. A., Tkachuk, M. J., Gretz, A. L., Fish, J. H., &Turpin, B. M. (1980). A two-process account of long-term serial position effects.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory,6, 355–369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Graf, P, Squire, L. R., &Mandler, G. (1984). The information that amnesic patients do not forget.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,10, 164–178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Greene, R. L. (l986a). Acommon basis for recency effects in iminediate and delayed recall.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,12, 413–418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Greene, R. L. (1986b). Sources of recency effects in free recall.Psychological Bulletin,99, 221–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Hintzman, D. L. (1991). Contingency analyses, hypotheses and artifacts: A reply to Flexser and to Gardiner.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,17, 341–345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Hintzman, D. L., &Hartry, A. L. (1990). Item effects in recognition and fragment completion: Contingency relations vary for different subsets of words.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,16, 955–969.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Hitch, G. J. (1985). Short-term memory and information processing in humans and animals: Towards an integrated framework. In L.-G. Nilsson & T. Archer (Eds.),Perspectives on learning and memory (pp. 119–136). London: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  37. Hitch, G. J., Rejman, M. H., & Turner, N. C. (1980, July).A new perspective on the recency effect. Paper presented at the meeting of the Experimental Psychology Society, Cambridge, England.Google Scholar
  38. Jacoby, L. L., &Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship between autobiographical memory and perceptual learning.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,110, 306–340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Katz, J., &Melzack, R. (1990). Pain “memories” in phantom limbs: Review and clinical observations.Pain,43, 319–336.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Kirsner, K., &Smith, M. C. (1974). Modality effects in word identification.Memory & Cognition,2, 637–640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. McKenzie, W. A., &Humphreys, M. S. (1991). Recency effects in direct and indirect memory tasks.Memory & Cognition,19, 321–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Melzack, R. (1992, April).Memory for pain. Paper presented at the meeting of the British Psychological Society, Scarborough, England.Google Scholar
  43. Mewaldt, S. P, Hinrichs, J. V., &Ghoneim, M. M. (1983). Diazepam and memory: Support for a duplex model of memory.Memory & Cognition,11, 557–564.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Monsell, S. (1985). Repetition and the lexicon. In A. W. Ellis (Ed.),Progress in the psychology of language (pp. 147–196). London: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  45. Murdock, B. B., Jr. (1963). Short-term retention of single paired associates.Journal of Experimental Psychology,68, 184–189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Parisi, G. (1986). A memory which forgets.Journal of Physics A: Mathematics & General,19, L617.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Parker, E. S., Schoenberg, R., Schwartz, B. L., &Tulving, E. (1983). Memories on the rising and falling blood-alcohol curve.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society,21, 363.Google Scholar
  48. Parkin, A. J., &Streete, S. (1988). Implicit and explicit memory in young children and adults.British Journal of Psychology,79, 361–369.Google Scholar
  49. Pinto, A. da C., &Baddeley, A. D. (1991). Where did you park your car? Analysis of a naturalistic long-term recency effect.European Journal of Cognitive Psychology,3, 297–313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Rejman, M. H. (1979). Recency and primacy effects in short-term memory. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Stirling, Stirling, Scotland.Google Scholar
  51. Richardson-Klavehn, A., &Bjork, R. A. (1988). Measures of memory.Annual Review of Psychology,39, 475–543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Rundus, D. (1971). Analysis of rehearsal processes in free recall.Journal of Experimental Psychology,89, 63–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Sanders, A. F. (1975). The foreperiod effect revisited.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,27, 591–598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Seamon, J. G., &Murray, P. (1976). Depth of processing in recall and recognition memory: Differential effects of stimulus meaningfulness and serial position.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory,2, 680–687.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Shallice, T., &Warrington, E. K. (1970). Independent functioning of verbal memory stores: A neuropsychological study.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,22, 261–273.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Squire, L. (1982). The neuropsychology of human memory.Annual Review of Neuroscience,5, 241–273.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Thurm, A. T., &Glanzer, M. (1971). Free recall in children: Longterm store vs short-term store.Psychonomic Science,23, 175–176.Google Scholar
  58. Tulving, E. (1968). Theoretical issues in free recall. In T. R. Dixon & D. L. Horton (Eds.),Verbal behavior and general behavior theory (pp. 2–36). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  59. Tulving, E., &Schacter, D. L. (1990). Priming and human memory systems.Science,247, 301–306.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Tulving, E, Schacter, D. L., &Stark, H. E. (1982). Priming effects in word fragment completion are independent of recognition memory.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,8, 336–342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Tzeng, O. J. L. (1973). Positive recency effects in delayed free recall.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,12, 436–439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Vallar, G., Papagno, C., &Baddeley, A. D. (1991). Long-term recency effects and phonological short-term memory: A neuropsychological case study.Cortex,27, 323–326.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Watkins, M. J., &Peynircioglu, Z. F. (1983). Three recency effects at the same time.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,22, 375–384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Waugh, N. C., &Norman, D. A. (1965). Primary memory.Psychological Review,72, 89–104.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Whitten, W. B. (1978). Output interference and long-term serial position effects.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory,4, 685–692.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Wilson, B. A., &Baddeley, A. D. (1988). Semantic and episodic memory in a post-meningitic amnesic patient.Brain & Cognition,8, 31–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 1993

Authors and Affiliations

  • Alan D. Baddeley
    • 1
  • Graham Hitch
    • 2
  1. 1.MRC Applied Psychology UnitCambridgeEngland
  2. 2.Lancaster UniversityLancasterEngland

Personalised recommendations