Advertisement

Behavior Research Methods & Instrumentation

, Volume 7, Issue 2, pp 165–173 | Cite as

System developments in the ACT language: Towards machine independence

  • J. R. Millenson
Session VII Contributed Papers: Physiological/Animal Learning Acquisition And Control Systems
  • 216 Downloads

Abstract

ACT (the Automated Contingency Translator) is a list-processing state-oriented sequence control language for on-line control and data acquisition of psychological experiments. Since 1967, ACT software has been progressively expanded and the machine base extended from the PDP8 family to the PDP9, NOVA, and the PDP11. The most recent variant of this language, designated ACT-N, removes previous arbitrary restrictions on state network complexity, expands the conversational repertoire, and adds a subset of compatible BASIC to ACT, thereby giving the package greatly increased computational powers and data storage facilities.

Keywords

Clock Cycle Basic Block Central Processor Print Statement Valley Electronics 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Fanna, M. V.Elementary BASIC with applications. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1970.Google Scholar
  2. Forster, J. M.List processing. London: MacDonald, 1967.Google Scholar
  3. Hare, V. C., Jr.Introduction to programming: A BASIC approach. New York: Harcourt, 1970.Google Scholar
  4. Lehigh Valley Electronics.The ACT-III primer. Computer language and hardware interface for controlling psychological experiments. Lehigh Valley Electronics, Fogelsville, Penna, 1970.Google Scholar
  5. McLean, R. S. PSYCHOL: A formal language for the control of psychological experiments. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, Carnegie-Mellon University, 1968.Google Scholar
  6. Millenson, J. R. An automatic contingency translator (ACT-I): A computer system for process control of psychological experimentation (Abstract). Psychonomic Bulletin, 1967, 1, 17.Google Scholar
  7. Millenson, J. R. A general language for on-line control of psychological experimentation. Proceedings of the DEC US Society, April 1968, 137–142.Google Scholar
  8. Millenson, J. R. Language and list structure of a compiler for experimental control. Computer Journal, 1970, 13, 340–343.Google Scholar
  9. Millenson, J. R. A programming language for on-line control of psychological experiments. Behavioral Science, 1971, 16, 248–256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Millenson, J. R. On-line sequential control of experiments by an automated contingency translator. In B. Weiss (Ed.)Digital computers in the behavioral laboratory. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1973. Pp. 1–39.Google Scholar
  11. Millenson, J. R., & Sullivan, G. D. A hardware random number generator for use with computer control of probabilistic contingencies. Behavior Research & Instrumentation, 1969, 1, 194–196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Poison, P. G. SCAT: Design criteria and software. Behavior Research Methods & Instrumentation, 1973, 5, 241–244.Google Scholar
  13. Snapper, A. G., Kadden, R. M., Knapp, J. Z., & Kushner, H. K. A notation system and computer program for behavioral experiments. DECUS Biomedical proceedings, June 1967.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 1975

Authors and Affiliations

  • J. R. Millenson
    • 1
  1. 1.McGilt UniversityMontrealCanada

Personalised recommendations