Skip to main content
Log in

Reasoning about conventional time as a function of conventional time systems

  • Published:
Memory & Cognition Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study investigated how reasoning about conventional time information varied as a function of conventional time systems by using the Chinese month and Jieqi systems. Twenty Chinese students were asked to answer month-related questions and another 20 were asked to answer Jieqi-related questions. Reaction time and accuracy were the dependent measures. A cross-boundary effect was observed in processing months, and distance and direction effects were obtained when participants judged the interval of Jieqi. These results suggested that arithmetic operations were used in Chinese reasoning about months and verbalarticulatory processes were used for the Jieqi. The effects of mode of language representation on cognition and the strategies for cross-linguistic study are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Altarriba, J. (Ed.) (1993).Cognition and culture: A cross-cultural approach to psychology. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Au, T. K. (1983). Chinese and English counterfactuals: The Sapir Whorf hypothesis revisited.Cognition,15, 155–187.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Au, T. K. (1984). Counterfactuals: In reply to Alfred Bloom.Cognition,17, 289–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Au, T. K. (1988). Language and cognition. In R. L. Schiefelbusch & L. L. Lloyd (Eds.),Language perspective: Acquisition, retardation, and intervention (2nd ed., pp. 125–146). Austin, TX: PRO-ED.

    Google Scholar 

  • Au, T. K. (1992). Cross-linguistic research on language and cognition: Methodological challenges. In H. Chen & O. J. L. Tzeng (Eds.),Language processing in Chinese (pp. 367–381). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, A. H. (1981).The linguistic shaping of thought: A study in the impact of language on thinking in China and the West. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, A. H. (1984). CautionThe words you use may affect what you say: A response to Au.Cognition,17, 275–287.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, C., &Stevenson, H. W. (1988). Cross-linguistic differences in digit span of preschool children.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,46, 150–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, C., &Stevenson, H. W. (1995). Motivation and mathematics achievement: A comparative study of Asian-American, Caucasian-American, and East Asian high school students.Child Development,66, 1215–1234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, H.-C., &Juola, J. F. (1982). Dimensions of lexical coding in Chinese and English.Memory & Cognition,10, 216–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, M. J. (1993). A comparison of Chinese and English language processing. In J. Altarriba (Ed.),Cognition and culture: A crosscultural approach to psychology (pp. 97–117). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H., Feltovich, P. J., &Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices.Cognitive Science,5, 121–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chincotta, D., &Underwood, G. (1997). Digit span and articulatory suppression: A cross-linguistic comparison.European Journal of Cognitive Psychology,9, 89–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dietrich, R., &Zheng, Z. X. (1993). The ordering of words in utterance production: An integrated view of the basic principles in German and Chinese. In J. Altarriba (Ed.),Cognition and culture: A crosscultural approach to psychology (pp. 119–143). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Eckensberger, L. H. (1973). Methodological issues of cross-cultural research in developmental psychology. In J. R. Nesselroade & H. W. Reese (Eds.),Life-span developmental psychology: Methodological issues (pp. 43–64). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, W. J. (1983). Image and verbal processes in reasoning about the months of the year.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,9, 650–666.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, W. J. (1989). The representation of temporal structure in children, adolescents and adults. In I. Levin & D. Zakay (Eds.),Time and human cognition: A life-span perspective (pp. 259–304). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hatano, G., Miyake, Y., &Binks, M. G. (1977). Performance of expert abacus operations.Cognition,5, 57–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatano, G., &Osawa, K. (1983). Digit memory of grand experts in abacus-derived mental calculation.Cognition,15, 95–110.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hoosain, R. (1982). Correlation between pronunciation speed and digit span size.Perceptual & Motor Skills,55, 1128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoosain, R. (1984). Experiments on digit spans in the Chinese and English languages. In H. S. R. Kao & R. Hoosain (Eds.),Psychological studies of the Chinese language (pp. 23–38). Hong Kong: Chinese Language Society of Hong Kong.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoosain, R. (1986). Language, orthography and cognitive processes: Chinese perspective for the SapirWhorf hypothesis.International Journal of Behavioral Development,9, 507–525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoosain, R. (1991).Psycholinguistic implications for linguistic relativity: A case study of Chinese. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huang, W. (1993). A model of Chinese adults reasoning about the months of the year.Psychological Science (China),15, 84–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hue, C.-W., &Erickson, J. R. (1988). Short-term memory for Chinese characters and radicals.Memory & Cognition,16, 196–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hung, D. L., &Tzeng, O. J. L. (1981). Orthographic variations and visual information processing.Psychological Bulletin,90, 377–414.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jiang, T., & Fang, G. (1995, August).Cognitive development of conventional time (week) of primary school pupils. Paper presented at the Asian-Pacific Region Psychological Conference, Guongzhou, China.

  • Kelly, M. K., Miller, K. F., Fang, G., &Feng, G. (1999). When days are numbered: Calendar structure and the development of calendar processing in English and Chinese.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,73, 289–314.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Larkin, J. H., McDermott, J., Simon, D. P., &Simon, H. A. (1980, June). Expert and novice performance in solving physics problems.Science,208, 1335–1342.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, K. F., &Paredes, D. (1996). On the shoulders of giants: Cultural tools and mathematical development. In R. Sternberg & T. Ben-Zeev (Eds.),The nature of mathematical thinking (pp. 83–117). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, K. F., Smith, C. M., Zhu, J., &Zhang, H. (1995). Preschool origins of cross-national differences in mathematical competence: The role of number-naming systems.Psychological Science,6, 56–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, K. F., &Stigler, J. W. (1987). Counting in Chinese: Cultural variation in a basic cognitive skill.Cognitive Development,2, 279–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, K. F., &Zhu, J. (1991). The trouble with teens: Accessing the structure of number names.Journal of Memory & Language,30, 48–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miura, I. T., Kim, C. C., Chang, C. M., &Okamoto, Y. (1988). Effects of language characteristics on childrens cognitive representation of number: Cross-national comparisons.Child Development,59, 1445–1450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miura, I. T., Okamoto, Y., Kim, C. C., Chang, C. M., Steere, M., &Fayol, M. (1994). Comparisons of childrens cognitive representation of number: China, France, Japan, Korea, Sweden, and the United States.International Journal of Behavioral Development,17, 401–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parise, F. (Ed.) (1982).The book of calendars. New York: Facts on File.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, G. B. (1982). Developing forms of arithmetical thought among the Oksapmin of Papua New Guinea.Developmental Psychology,18, 583–594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saxe, G. B. (1985). Effects of schooling on arithmetical understandings: Studies with Oksapmin children in Papua New Guinea.Journal of Educational Psychology,77, 503–513.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, H. W., Chen, C., &Lee, S. Y. (1993, January). Mathematics achievement of Chinese, Japanese, and American children: Ten years later.Science,259, 53–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stigler, J. W. (1984). Mental abacus: The effect of abacus training on Chinese childrens mental calculation.Cognitive Psychology,16, 145–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stigler, J. W., Lee, S. Y., &Stevenson, H. W. (1986). Digit memory in Chinese and English: Evidence for a temporally limited store.Cognition,23, 1–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Wei Huang.

Additional information

This research was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (39470247). The study also received the 1997 APS Student Caucus Research Competition Award.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Huang, W. Reasoning about conventional time as a function of conventional time systems. Mem Cogn 27, 1080–1086 (1999). https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03201237

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03201237

Keywords

Navigation