Skip to main content
Log in

Priming in a free association task as a function of association directionality

  • Published:
Memory & Cognition Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two experiments investigated priming in free association, a conceptual implicit memory task. The stimuli consisted of bidirectionally associated word pairs (e.g.,beach-sand) and unidirectionally associated word pairs that have no association from the target response back to the stimulus cue (e.g.,bone-dog). In the study phase, target words (e.g.,sand, dog) were presented in an incidental learning task. In the test phase, participants generated an associate to the stimulus cues (e.g.,beach, bone). In both experiments, priming was obtained for targets (e.g.,sand) that had an association back to the cue, butnot for targets (e.g.,dog) for which such a backward association was absent. These results are problematic for theoretical accounts that attribute priming in free association to the strengthening oftarget responses. It is argued that priming in free association depends on the strengthening of cuetargetassociations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson, J. R. (1983). A spreading activation theory of memory.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,22, 261–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Basden, B. H., Basden, D. R., &Gargano, G. J. (1993). Directed forgetting in implicit and explicit memory tasks: A comparison of methods.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,19, 603–616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blaxton, T. A. (1989). Investigating dissociations among memory measures: Support for a transfer appropriate processing framework. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,15, 657–668.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burnage, G. (1990).CELEX: A guide for users. Nijmegen: SSN.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cabeza, R. (1994). A dissociation between two implicit conceptual tests supports the distinction between types of conceptual processing.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,1, 505–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Groot, A. M. B. (1980).Mondelinge Woordassociationormen: 100 woordassociaties op 460 Nederlandse zelfstandige naamwoorden [Oral word association norms: 100 word associations to 460 Dutch nouns]. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, R., &Rozin, P. (1984). Activation of existing memories in anterograde amnesia.Journal of Abnormal Psychology,93, 98–105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Graf, P., &Mandler, G. (1984). Activation makes words more accessible, but not necessarily more retrievable.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,23, 553–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graf, P., Shimamura, A. P., &Squire, L. R. (1985). Priming across modalities and priming across category levels: Extending the domain of preserved function in amnesia.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,11, 386–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graf, P., Squire, L. R., &Mandler, G. (1984). The information that amnesic patients do not forget.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,10, 164–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, M. S., Bain, J. D., &Pike, R. (1989). Different ways to cue a coherent memory system: A theory for episodic, semantic and procedural tasks.Psychological Review,96, 208–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L., &Dallas, M. (1981). On the relationship between autobiographicalPsychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,13, 306–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kučera, H., &Francis, W. N. (1967).Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lauteslager, M., Schaap, T., &Schievels, D. (1986).Schriftelijke woordassociatienormen voor 549 Nederlandse zelfstandige naamwoorden [Written word association norms for 549 Dutch nouns]. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNamara, T. P. (1992). Theories of priming: I. Associative distance and lag.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,18, 1173–1190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J. (1969). Interaction of information in word recognition.Psychological Review,76, 163–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Schreiber, T. A. (1994).The University of South Florida word association, rhyme and word fragment norms. Unpublished manuscript.

  • Rappold, V. A., &Hashtroudi, S. (1991). Does organization improve priming?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,17, 103–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., III (1990). Implicit memory: Retention without remembering.American Psychologist,45, 1043–1056.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., III, &McDermott, K. B. (1993). implicit memory in normal human subjects. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.),Handbook of neuropsychology (Vol. 8, pp. 63–131). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., III,Weldon, M. S., Stadler, M. L., &Riegler, G. L. (1992). Direct comparison of two implicit memory tests: Word fragment and word stem completion.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,18, 1251–1269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salasoo, A., Shiffrin, R. M., &Feustel, T. C. (1985). Building permanent memory codes: Codification and repetition effects in word identification.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,114, 50–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shimamura, A. P., &Squire, L. R. (1984). Paired-associate learning and priming in amnesia: A neuropsychological study.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,113, 556–570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Storms, L. H. (1958). Apparent backward associations: A situational effect.Journal of Experimental Psychology,55, 390–395.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vaidya, C. J., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Keane, M. M., &Monti, L. A. (1995). Perceptual and conceptual memory processes in global amnesia.Neuropsychology,9, 580–591.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaidya, C. J., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Keane, M. M., Monti, L. A., Gutiérrez-Rivas, H., Zarella, M. M. (1997). Evidence for multiple mechanisms of conceptual priming on implicit memory tests.Journal of experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition,23, 1324–1343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Loon-Vervoorn, W. A., &Van Bekkum, I. J. (1991).Woordassociatie lexicon [Word association lexicon]. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weldon, M. S., &Coyote, K. C. (1996). Failure to find the word picture superiority effect in implicit conceptual memory tests.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,22, 670–686.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to René Zeelenberg.

Additional information

The research reported in this article was supported by Grant 575-56072 of the Foundation for Behavioral and Educational Sciences of the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Zeelenberg, R., Shiffrin, R.M. & Raaijmakers, J.G.W. Priming in a free association task as a function of association directionality. Mem Cogn 27, 956–961 (1999). https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03201226

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03201226

Keywords

Navigation