Advertisement

Memory & Cognition

, Volume 26, Issue 3, pp 449–462 | Cite as

Retrieval dynamics in recognition and list discrimination: Further evidence of separate processes of familiarity and recall

  • Douglas L. Hintzman
  • David A. Caulton
  • Daniel J. Levitin
Article

Abstract

Two experiments tested the hypothesis that the time course of retrieval from memory is different for familiarity and recall. The response-signal method was used to compare memory retrieval dynamics in yes-no recognition memory, as a measure of familiarity, with those of list discrimination, as a measure of contextual recall. Responses were always made with regard to membership in two previous study lists. In Experiment 1 an exclusion task requiring positive responses to words from one list and negative responses to new words and words from the nontarget list was used. In Experiment 2, recognition and list discrimination were separate tasks. Retrieval curves from both experiments were consistent, showing that the minimal retrieval time for recognition was about 100 msec faster than that for list discrimination. Repetition affected asymptotic performance but had no reliable effects on retrieval dynamics in either the recognition or the list-discrimination task.

Keywords

Journal ofExperimental Psychology Study List Test Word Test List Target List 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Aggleton, J. P., &Shaw, C. (1996). Amnesia and recognition memory: A reanalysis of psychometric data.Neuropsychologia,34, 51–62.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Anderson, J. R., &Bower, G. H. (1972). Recognition and retrieval processes in free recall.Psychological Review,79, 97–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Anderson, J. R., &Bower, G. H. (1974). Interference in memory for multiple contexts.Memory & Cognition,2, 509–514.Google Scholar
  4. Atkinson, R. C., &Juola, J. F. (1973). Factors influencing speed and accuracy of word recognition. In S. Kornblum (Ed.),Attention and performance IV (pp. 583–612). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  5. Barnett, V., &Lewis, T. (1984).Outliers in statistical data (2nd ed.). Chichester, U.K.: Wiley.Google Scholar
  6. Buchner, A., Erdfelder, E., Steffens, M. C., &Martensen, H. (1997). The nature of memory processes underlying recognition judgments in the process dissociation procedure.Memory & Cognition,25, 508–517.Google Scholar
  7. Clark, S. E., &Gronlund, S. D. (1996). Global matching models of recognition memory: How the models match the data.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,3, 37–60.Google Scholar
  8. Corbett, A. T., &Wickelgren, W. A. (1978). Semantic memory retrieval: Analysis by speed accuracy tradeoff functions.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,30, 1–15.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Curran, T., &Hintzman, D. L. (1995). Violations of the independence assumption in process dissociation.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,21, 531–547.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Curran, T., &Hintzman, D. L. (1997). Consequences and causes of correlations in process dissociation.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,23, 496–504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Dosher, B. A. (1981). The effects of delay and interference: A speedaccuracy study.Cognitive Psychology,13, 551–582.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dosher, B. A. (1984a). Degree of learning and retrieval speed: Study time and multiple exposures.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,10, 541–574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Dosher, B. A. (1984b). Discriminating preexperimental (semantic) from learned (episodic) associations: A speed-accuracy study.Cognitive Psychology,16, 519–555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Dosher, B. A., &Rosedale, G. (1991). Judgments of semantic and episodic relatedness: Common time-course and failure of segregation.Journal of Memory & Language,30, 125–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Eich, J. M. (1982). A composite holographic associative recall model.Psychological Review,89, 627–661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Eichenbaum, H., Otto, T., &Cohen, N. J. (1994). Two functional components of the hippocampal memory system.Behavioral & Brain Sciences,17, 449–518.Google Scholar
  17. Gillund, G., &Shiffrin, R. M. (1984). A retrieval model for both recognition and recall.Psychological Review,91, 1–67.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Gronlund, S. D., Edwards, M. B., &Ohrt, D. D. (1997). Comparison of the retrieval of item versus spatial position information.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,23, 1261–1274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gronlund, S. D., &Ratcliff, R. (1989). The time-course of item and associative information: Implications for global memory models.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,15, 846–858.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Heil, M., Rösler, F., &Hennighausen, E. (1996). Topographically distinct cortical activation in episodic long-term memory: The retrieval of spatial versus verbal information.Memory & Cognition,24, 777–795.Google Scholar
  21. Hintzman, D. L. (1986). “Schema abstraction” in a multiple-trace memory model.Psychological Review,93, 411–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hintzman, D. L. (1988). Judgments of frequency and recognition memory in a multiple-trace memory model.Psychological Review,95, 528–551.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hintzman, D. L., &Block, R. A. (1971). Repetition and memory: Evidence for a multiple-trace hypothesis.Journal of Experimental Psychology,88, 297–306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hintzman, D. L., Block, R. A., &Summers, J. J. (1973). Contextual associations and memory for serial position.Journal of Experimental Psychology,97, 220–229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hintzman, D. L., &Caulton, D. A. (1997). Recognition memory and modality judgments: A comparison of retrieval dynamics.Journal of Memory & Language,37, 1–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Hintzman, D. L., Caulton, D. A., &Curran, T. (1994). Retrieval constraints and the mirror effect.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,20, 275–289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hintzman, D. L., &Curran, T. (1994). Retrieval dynamics of recognition and frequency judgments: Evidence for separate processes of familiarity and recall.Journal of Memory & Language,33, 1–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Hintzman, D. L., &Curran, T. (1997a). Comparing retrieval dynamics in recognition memory and lexical decision.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,126, 228–247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hintzman, D. L., &Curran, T. (1997b). More than one way to violate independence: Reply to Jacoby and Shrout.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,23, 511–513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hintzman, D. L., &Waters, R. M. (1970). Recency and frequency as factors in list discrimination.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,9, 218–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Humphreys, M. S., Bain, J. D., &Pike, R. (1989). Different ways to cue a coherent memory system: A theory for episodic, semantic, and procedural tasks.Psychological Review,96, 208–233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory.Journal of Memory & Language,30, 513–541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Jacoby, L. L., Begg, I. M., &Toth, J. P. (1997). In defense of functional independence: Violations of assumptions underlying the processdissociation procedure?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,23, 484–495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Jacoby, L. L., &Shrout, P. E. (1997). Toward a psychometric analysis of violations of the independence assumption in process dissociation.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,23, 505–510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Jacoby, L. L., Toth, J. P., &Yonelinas, A. P. (1993). Separating conscious and unconscious influences of memory: Measuring recollection.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,122, 139–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Janowsky, J. S., Shimamura, A. P., &Squire, L. R. (1989). Source memory impairment in patients with frontal lobe lesions.Neuropsychologia,27, 1043–1056.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Johnson, M. K., Hashtroudi, S., &Lindsay, D. S. (1993). Source monitoring.Psychological Bulletin,114, 3–28.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Libet, B. (1993).Neurophysiology of consciousness: Selected papers and new essays of Benjamin Libet. Boston: Birkhauser.Google Scholar
  39. Macmillan, N. A., &Creelman, C. D. (1991).Detection theory: A user’s guide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  40. Macmillan, N. A., &Creelman, C. D. (1996). Triangles in ROC space: History and theory of “nonparametric” measures of sensitivity and response bias.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,3, 164–170.Google Scholar
  41. Marr, D. (1971). Simple memory: A theory for archicortex.Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London,262, 23–81.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. McElree, B., &Dosher, B. A. (1993). Serial retrieval processes in the recovery of order information.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,122, 291–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Milner, B., Corsi, P., &Leonard, G. (1991). Frontal-lobe contribution to recency judgments.Neuropsychologia,29, 601–618.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Moscovitch, M. (1992). Memory and working-with-memory: A component process model based on modules and central systems.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,4, 257–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Mulligan, N., &Hirshman, E. (1995). Speed-accuracy trade-offs and the dual process model of recognition memory.Journal of Memory & Language,34, 1–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Murdock, B. B., Jr. (1982). A theory for the storage and retrieval of item and associative information.Psychological Review,89, 609–626.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Nusbaum, H. C., Pisoni, D. B., &Davis, C. K. (1984).Sizing up the Hoosier mental lexicon: Measuring the familiarity of 20,000 words (Progress Rep. No. 10). Bloomington: Indiana University, Department of Psychology, Speech Research Laboratory.Google Scholar
  48. Raaijmakers, J. G. W., &Shiffrin, R. M. (1992). Models for recall and recognition.Annual Review of Psychology,43, 205–234.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Ratcliff, R., &McKoon, G. (1989). Similarity information versus relational information: Differences in the time course of retrieval.Cognitive Psychology,21, 139–155.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Reed, A. V. (1973). Speed-accuracy tradeoff in recognition memory.Science,181, 574–576.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Reed, A. V. (1976). List length and the time-course of recognition in immediate memory.Memory & Cognition,4, 16–30.Google Scholar
  52. Rensink, R. A. (1990). Toolbox-based routines for Macintosh timing and display.Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers,22, 105–107.Google Scholar
  53. Snodgrass, J. G., &Corwin, J. (1988). Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: Application to dementia and amnesia.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,117, 34–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Treves, A., &Rolls, E. T. (1993). Computational analysis of the role of the hippocampus in memory.Hippocampus,4, 374–391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Wickelgren, W. A. (1977). Speed-accuracy tradeoff and information processing dynamics.Acta Psychologica,41, 67–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Wickelgren, W. A. (1981). Human learning and memory. In M. R. Rosenzweig & L. W. Porter (Eds.),Annual review of psychology (Vol. 32, pp. 21–52). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.Google Scholar
  57. Wickelgren, W. A., &Corbett, A. T. (1977). Associative interference and retrieval dynamics in yes-no recall and recognition.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,3, 189–202.Google Scholar
  58. Winograd, E. (1968). List differentiation as a function of frequency and retention interval.Journal of Experimental Psychology Monographs,76, (2, Pt. 2).Google Scholar
  59. Yonelinas, A. P. (1994). Receiver-operating characteristics in recognition memory: Evidence for a dual-process model.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,20, 1341–1354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Yonelinas, A. P., &Jacoby, L. L. (1994). Dissociations of processes in recognition memory: Effects of interference and of response speed.Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology,48, 516–534.PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 1998

Authors and Affiliations

  • Douglas L. Hintzman
    • 1
  • David A. Caulton
    • 1
  • Daniel J. Levitin
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of OregonEugene

Personalised recommendations