Animal Learning & Behavior

, Volume 14, Issue 4, pp 339–348 | Cite as

The evolution of learned and innate behavior: Contributions from genetics and neurobiology to a theory of behavioral evolution

  • Ann Jane Tierney
Article

Abstract

In recent years, ethologists and psychologists have become increasingly interested in the evolution of the ability to learn and in the relationship between innate and learned behavior. However, recent discussions of behavioral evolution have not adequately incorporated contemporary knowledge of nervous system development and structure. Most discussions are based on the following assumptions: (1) That innate behaviors are programmed by specific genes; (2) that learning requires a larger, more flexible nervous system than does innate behavior; and (3) that the ability to learn is phylogenetically more recent than innate behavior. This paper reviews information about nervous system development and the neurobiology of plasticity and learning that questions the validity of these assumptions. It is hypothesized that behavioral flexibility is phylogenetically primitive and that learned behavioral adaptations may commonly precede innate forms of the same behaviors. The role of genetic assimilation in behavioral evolution is discussed.

References

  1. Alcock, J. (1979).Animal behavior: An evolutionary approach. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.Google Scholar
  2. Alkon, D. L. (1980). Cellular analysis of a gastropod (Hermissenda crassicomis) model of associative learning.Biological Bulletin,159, 505–560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Alkon, D. L. (1984). Changes in membrane currents during learning.Journal of Experimental Biology,112, 95–112.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Baldwin, J. M. (1896). A new factor in evolution.American Naturalist,30, 441–451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Barash, D. P. (1977).Sociobiology and behavior. New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  6. Bateson, P. O. G. (1971). Imprinting. In H. Moltz (Ed.),The ontogeny of vertebrate behavior (pp. 369–387). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  7. Bateson, P. P. G. (1984). Genes, evolution and learning. In P. Marler & H. S. Terrace (Eds.),Dahlem workshop on the biology of learning (pp. 75–88). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  8. Catchpole, C. K. (1982). The evolution of bird sounds in relation to mating and spacing behavior. In D. E. Kroodsma & E. H. Miller (Eds.),Acoustic communication in birds (Vol. 1, pp. 297–319). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  9. Changeux, J. P., &Danchin, A. (1976). Selective stabilization of developing synapses as a mechanism for the specification of neuronal networks.Nature,264, 705–712.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Cotman, C. W., &Nieto-Sampedro, M. (1982). Brain function, synapse renewal, and plasticity.Annual Review of Psychology,33, 371–401.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Cotman, C. W., &Nieto-Sampedro, M. (1984). Cell biology of synaptic plasticity.Science,225, 1287–1294.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Cotman, C. W., Nieto-Sampedro, M., &Harris, E. W. (1981). Synapse replacement in the nervous system of adult vertebrates.Physiological Review,61, 684–784.Google Scholar
  13. Couvillon, P. A., &Bitterman, M. E. (1980). Some phenomena of associative learning in honeybees.Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology,94, 878–885.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Cowan, W. M. (1973). Neuronal death as a regulative mechanism in the control of cell number in the nervous system. In M. Rockstein (Ed.),Development and aging in the nervous system (pp. 19–41). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  15. Cowan, W. M. (1979). Selection and control in neurogenesis. In F. O. Schmitt & F. C. Worden (Eds.),Neurosciences fourth study program (pp. 59–79). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  16. Dittos, W. P. J., &Lemon, R. E. (1969). Effects of song tutoring and acoustic isolation on the song repertoires of cardinals.Animal Behaviour,17, 523–533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Ehrman, L., &Parsons, P. A. (1981).Behavior genetics and evolution. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  18. Garcia, J., &Koelling, R. A. (1966). Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning.Psychonomic Science,4, 123–124.Google Scholar
  19. Goodman, C. S. (1979). Isogenic grasshoppers. Genetic variability and development of identified neurons. In X. Breakefield (Ed.),Neurogenetics: Genetic approaches to the nervous system (pp. 101–151). New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  20. Gould, J. L. (1982).Ethology: The mechanics and evolution of behavior. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
  21. Gould, J. L., &Marler, P. (1984). Ethology and the natural history of learning. In P. Marler & H. S. Terrace (Eds.),Dahlem workshop on the biology of learning (pp. 47–74). New York-Springer.Google Scholar
  22. Greenough, W. T. (1984). Structural correlates of information storage in the mammalian brain: A review and hypothesis.Trends in Neuroscience,73, 229–233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Guillery, R. W. (1974). Visual pathways in albinos.Scientific American,230, 44–54.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Hailman, J. P. (1967). The ontogeny of an instinct.Behaviour Supplements,15, 1–159.Google Scholar
  25. Hailman, J. P. (1969). How an instinct is learned.Scientific American,111, 98–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Haldane, J. B. S. (1959) Natural selection. In P. R. Bell (Ed.),Darwin’s Biological Work (pp. 101–149). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  27. Hall, J. C., &Greenspan, R. J. (1979). Genetic analysis ofDrosophila neurobiology.Annual Review of Genetics,13, 127–195.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Hamburger, V., &Oppenheim, R. W. (1982). Naturally occurring neuronal death in vertebrates.Neuroscience Commentary,1, 39–55.Google Scholar
  29. Harris, W. A. (1981). Neural activity and development.Annual Review of Physiology,43, 689–710.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Hawkins, R. D., Abrams, T. W., Carew, T. J., &Kandel, E. R. (1983). A cellular mechanism of classical conditioning inAplysia. Activity-dependent amplification of presynaptic facilitation.Science,219, 400–405.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Hinde, R. A. (1970).Animal behaviour. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  32. Hollyday, M., &Hamburger, V. (1976). Reduction of the naturally occurring motor neuron loss by enlargement of the periphery.Journal of Comparative Neurology,170, 311–320.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Hubel, D. H., &Wiesel, T. N. (1971). Aberrant visual projections in the Siamese cat.Journal of Physiology,218, 33–62.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Jacobson, M. (1978).Developmental neurobiology. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  35. Johnston, T. D. (1981). Contrasting approaches to a theory of learning.Behavioral & Brain Sciences,4, 125–173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Johnston, T. D. (1982). Selective costs and benefits in the evolution of learning.Advances in the Study of Behavior,12, 65–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Kandel, E. R. (1981). Synaptic transmission II: Presynaptic factors controlling transmitter release. In E. R. Kandel & J. H. Schwartz (Eds.),Principles of neural science. New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  38. Kandel, E. R., &Schwartz, J. H. (1982). Molecular biology of learning: Modulation of transmitter release.Science,218, 433–443.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. King, A. P., &West, M. J. (1977). Species identification in the North American cowbird: Appropriate responses to abnormal song.Science,195, 1002–1004.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Kroodsma, D. E. (1982). Learning and the ontogeny of sound signals in birds. In D. E. Kroodsma & E. H. Miller (Eds.),Acoustic communication in birds (pp. 1–23). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  41. Lanyon, W. E. (1979). Development of song in the wood thrush (Hylocichla mustelina), with notes on a technique for hand-rearing passerine birds from the egg.American Museum Novitates,2666, 1–27.Google Scholar
  42. Lehrman, D. S. (1953). A critique of Konrad Lorenz’s theory of instinctive behaviour.Quarterly Review of Biology,28, 337–363.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Levinthal, F., Macagno, E., &Levinthal, C. (1975). Anatomy and development of identified cells in isogenic organisms.Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology,40, 321–331.Google Scholar
  44. Lynch, G., &Baudry, M. (1984). The biochemistry of memory: A new and specific hypothesis.Science,224, 1057–1063.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Macagno, E. R., Lopresti, V., &Levinthal, C. (1973). Structure and development of neuronal connections in isogenic organisms: Variation and similarities in the optic system ofDaphnia magna.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,70, 57–61.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Marler, P. A. (1970). A comparative approach to vocal learning: Song development in the white-crowned sparrow.Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology,71, 1–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Marler, P., &Mundinger, P. (1971). Vocal learning in birds. In H. Moltz (Ed.),The ontogeny of vertebrate behavior (pp. 389–450). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  48. Maturana, H. R., Lettvin, J. Y., McCulloch, W. S., &Pitts, W. H. (1960). Anatomy and physiology of vision in the frog (Rana pipiens).Journal of General Physiology,43, 129–175.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Mayr, E. (1963).Animal species and evolution. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  50. Mayr, E. (1974). Behavior programs and evolutionary strategies.American Scientist,62, 650–659.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. McGuire, T. R., &Hirsch, J. (1977). Behavior-genetic analysis ofPhormia regina: Conditioning, reliable individual differences, and selection.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,74, 5193–5197.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Morgan, C. L. (1896).Habit and instinct. London: Arnold.Google Scholar
  53. Nottebohm, F. (1972). The origins of vocal learning.American Naturalist,106, 116–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Osborn, H. F. (1896). A mode of evolution requiring neither natural selection nor the inheritance of acquired characters.Transactions of the New York Academy of Sciences,15, 141–142.Google Scholar
  55. Palka, J., &Edwards, J. S. (1974). The cerci and abdominal giant fibers of the house cricket,Acheta domesticus. II. Regeneration and effects of chronic deprivation.Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B,185, 105–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Partridge, L. (1983) Genetics and behaviour. In T. R. Halliday & P. J. B. Slater (Eds.),Genes, development and behavior (pp. 11–51). New York: Freeman.Google Scholar
  57. Piaget, J. (1978).Behavior and evolution. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
  58. Plotkin, H. C., &Odling-Smee, F. J. (1979). Learning, change and evolution: An enquiry into the teleonomy of learning.Advances in the Study of Behavior,10, 1–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Roper, T. J. (1983). Learning as a biological phenomenon. In T. R. Halliday & P. J. B. Slater (Eds.),Genes, development and behavior (pp. 178–212). New York Freeman.Google Scholar
  60. Rozin, P. (1976). The evolution of intelligence and access to the cognitive unconscious.Progress in Psychobiology & Physiological Psychology,6, 245–280.Google Scholar
  61. Rozin, P., & Schull, J. (in press). The adaptive-evolutionary point of view in experimental psychology: A critical analysis of “constraints.” In R. C. Atkinson, R. J. Herrnstein, G. Lindzey, & R. D. Luce (Eds.),Handbook of experimental psychology. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  62. Sahley, C., Rudy, J. W., &Gelperin, A. (1981). An analysis of associative learning in a terrestrial mollusc.Journal of Comparative Physiology,144, 1–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Shashoua, V. E. (1982). Molecular and cell biological aspects of learning: Towards a theory of memory.Advances in Cellular Neurobiology,3, 97–141.Google Scholar
  64. Shashoua, V. E. (1985). The role of extracellular proteins in learning and memory.American Scientist,73, 364–370.Google Scholar
  65. Shepherd, G. M. (1983).Neurobiology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  66. Skrede, K. K., &Malthe-Sorenssen, D. (1981). Increased resting and evoked release of transmitter following repetitive electrical tetanization in hippocampus: A biochemical correlate to long-lasting synaptic potentiation.Brain Research,208, 436–441.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Slater, P. J. B. (1983). Bird song learning: Theme and variations. In A. H. Brush & G. A. Clark (Eds.),Perspectives in ornithology (pp. 475–511). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Staddon, J. E. R. (1983).Adaptive behavior and learning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  69. Stent, G. (1981). Strength and weakness of the genetic approach to the development of the nervous system. In W. M. Cowan (Ed.),Studies in developmental neurobiology (pp. 288–321). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  70. Swanson, L. W., Teyler, T. J., &Thompson, R. F. (1982). Hippocampal long-term potentiation: Mechanisms and implications for memory.Neurosciences Research Program Bulletin,20, 617–764.Google Scholar
  71. Thompson, R. F., Berger, T. W., &Madden, I., IV (1983). Cellular processes of learning and memory in the mammalian CNS.Annual Review of Neuroscience,6, 447–491.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Tinbergen, N., &Perdeck, A. C. (1950). On the stimulus situations releasing the begging response in the newly hatched herring gull (Larus argentatus Pont.).Behaviour,3, 1–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Truman, J. W. (1984). Cell death in invertebrate nervous systems.Annual Review of Neuroscience,7, 171–188.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Tsukahara, N. (1981). Synaptic plasticity in the mammalian central nervous system.Annual Review of Neuroscience,4, 351–379.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. Waddington, C. H. (1953). Genetic assimilation of an acquired characterEvolution,7, 118–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Waddington, C. H. (1956). Genetic assimilation of the bithorax phenotypeEvolution,10, 1–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Waddington, C. H. (1961). Genetic assimilation.Advances in Genetics,10, 257–293.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. Ward, S., Thomson, N., White, J. C., &Brenner, S. (1975). Electron microscopical reconstruction of the anterior anatomy of the nematodeCaeanorhabditis elegans Journal of Comparative Neurology,160, 313–338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Williams, G. C. (1966).Adaptation and natural selection Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 1986

Authors and Affiliations

  • Ann Jane Tierney
    • 1
  1. 1.University of TorontoTorontoCanada

Personalised recommendations