Advertisement

Animal Learning & Behavior

, Volume 14, Issue 3, pp 322–330 | Cite as

Object permanence in the African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus)

  • Irene M. Pepperberg
  • Florence A. Kozak
Article

Abstract

An African Grey parrot was assessed for object permanence using tasks designed for human infants. The subject showed concepts of object permanence directly comparable to primate and nonprimate mammalian species similarly tested. Because the subject has also been the focus of a study on interspecies communication, the results are discussed in terms of the possible relationship between sensorimotor intelligence and language-like behaviors.

Keywords

Domestic Chicken Object Permanence Invisible Displacement Visual Pursuit Visible Displacement 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Antinucci, F., Spinozzi, R., Visalberghi, E., &Volterra, V. (1982). Cognitive development in a Japanese macaque (Macaca fuscata).Istituto Superiore di Sanità. Annali.,2, 177–184.Google Scholar
  2. Ault, R. L. (1983).Children’s cognitive development. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  3. Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., &Volterra, V. (1979).The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  4. Bateson, P. (1979). How do sensitive periods arise and what are they for?Animal Behaviour,27, 470–486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bossema, I. (1968). Recovery of acorns in the European jay.Koninklijke Nederlandse Akademie Van Wetenschappen.Proceedings Series C,71, 1–5.Google Scholar
  6. Bossema, I. (1979). Jays and oaks: An eco-ethological study of a symbiosis.Behaviour,70, 1–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bower, T. G. R. (1982).Development in infancy. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
  8. Braggio, J. T., Hall, A. D., Buchanan, J. P., &Nadler, R. D. (1982). Logical and illogical errors made by apes and children on a cognitive task.Journal of Human Evolution,11, 159–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bruner, J. S. (1964). The course of cognitive development.American Psychologist,19, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Chardard-Segurel, C. (1984).Possibilités d’abstraction de différentes qualités sensibles chez le perroquet gris (Psittacus erithacus,)en référence à l’enfant humain. Doctoral dissertation, University of the Sorbonne, Paris.Google Scholar
  11. Chardard-Segurel, C. (1985).Cognitive capacities of the African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus)on classification problems. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  12. Chavalier-Skolnikoff, S. (1976). The ontogeny of primate intelligence and its implications for communicative potential: A preliminary report.Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,280, 173–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Chevalier-Skolnikoff, S. (1981). The Clever Hans phenomenon, cuing and ape signing: A Piagetian analysis of methods for instructing animals.Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,364, 60–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Davis, H. (1984). Discrimination of the number three by a raccoon (Procyon lotor).Animal Learning & Behavior,12, 409–413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Etienne, A. S. (1973). Developmental stages and cognitive structures as determinants of what is learned. In R. A. Hinde & J. Stevenson (Eds.),Constraints on learning. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  16. Etienne, A. S. (1984). The meaning of object permanence at different zoological levels.Human Development,27, 309–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Flavell, J. H. (1985).Cognitive development. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  18. Gardner, B. T., &Gardner, R. A. (1975). Evidence for sentence constituents in early utterances of child and chimpanzee.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,104, 244–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Ginsburg, H., &Opper, S. (1969)Piaget’s theory of intellectual development: An introduction. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  20. Gruber, H. E., Girgus, J. S., &Banuazizi, A. (1971). The development of object permanence in the cat.Developmental Psychology,4, 9–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Hall, A. D., Braggio, J. T., Buchanan, J. P., Nadler, R. D., Karan, D., &Sams, J. B. (1980). Multiple classification performance of juvenile chimpanzees, normal children, and retarded children.International Journal of Primatology,1, 345–359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hallock, M. B., &Worobey, J. (1984). Cognitive development in chimpanzee infants (Pan troglodytes).Journal of Human Evolution,13, 441–447.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Harris, P. L. (1983). Infant cognition. In M. M. Haith & J. J. Campos (Eds.),Handbook of child psychology: Infancy and developmental psychobiology (Vol. 2). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  24. Hayes, K. J., &Nissen, C. H. (1971). Higher mental functions of a home-raised chimpanzee. In A. Schrier & F. Stollnitz (Eds.),Behavior of nonhuman primates (Vol 4). New York: Academic Press. (Original work published 1956).Google Scholar
  25. Held, R. (1965). Plasticity in sensory-motor systems.Scientific American,213, 84–94.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Herman, L., Richards, D. G., &Wolz, J. P. (1984). Comprehension of sentences by bottlenosed dolphinsCognition,16, 129–219.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hockett, C. (1960). Logical considerations in the study of animal communication. In W. E. Lanyon & W. N. Tavolga (Eds.),Animal sounds and communication. Washington, DC: American Institute of Biological Science.Google Scholar
  28. Hodos, W. (1982). Some perspectives on the evolution of intelligence and the brain. In D. R. Griffin (Ed.),Animal mind-human mind. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
  29. Kamil, A. C., &Balda, R. P. (1985). Cache recovery and spatial memory in Clark’s nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana).Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes,11, 95–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Kamil, A. C., &Hunter, M. W., III. (1970). Performance on object discrimination learning set by the Greater Hill mynah,Gracula religiosa.Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology,13, 68–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kroodsma, D. E. (1982). Learning and the ontogeny of sound signals in birds. In D. E. Kroodsma & E. H. Miller (Eds.),Acoustic communication in birds: Vol. 2. Song learning and its consequences. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  32. Krushinskii, L. V. (1960).Animal behavior: Its normal and abnormal development. New York: Consultants Bureau.Google Scholar
  33. Landers, W. F. (1968).The effects of different amounts and types of experience on infant object concepts. Doctoral dissertation, University of Houston. (University Microfilms No. 69-775)Google Scholar
  34. Lea, S. E. G. (1984). Complex general process learning in nonmammalian vertebrates. In P. Marler & H. S. Terrace (Eds.),The biology of learning. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
  35. Lieberman, P. (1984).The biology and evolution of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  36. Marler, P., Duffy, A., &Pickert, R. (1986a). Vocal communication in the domestic chicken. I. Does a sender communicate information about the quality of a food referent to a receiver?Animal Behaviour,43, 188–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Marler, P., Duffy, A., &Pickert, R. (1986b). Vocal communication in the domestic chicken. II. Is a sender sensitive to the presence and nature of a receiver?Animal Behaviour,34, 194–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Mathieu, M., Bouchard, M-A., Granger, L., &Herscovitch, J. (1976). Piagetian object-permanence inCebus capucinus, Lagothrica flavicauda, andPan troglodytes.Animal Behaviour,24, 585–588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Moran, G., Joch, E., & Sorenson, L (1983, June)The response of meerkats (Suncata suricatta)to changes in olfactory cues on established scent posts. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Animal Behavior Society, Lewisburg, PAGoogle Scholar
  40. Parker, S. T. (1977) Piaget’s sensorimotor series in an infant macaque A model for comparing unstereotyped behavior and intelligence in human and nonhuman primates. In S. Chevalier-Skolnikoff & F. E. Poirier (Eds.),Primate biosocial development. New York: Garland.Google Scholar
  41. Pepperberg, I. M. (1979, June).Functional word use in an African Grey parrot Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Animal Behavior Society, New Orleans.Google Scholar
  42. Pepperberg, I. M. (1981). Functional vocalizations by an African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus).Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie,55, 139–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Pepperberg, I. M. (1983). Cognition in the African Grey parrot: Preliminary evidence for auditory/vocal comprehension of the class concept.Animal Learning & Behavior,11, 179–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Pepperberg, I. M. (1986, June).Categorization and second-order concepts in the African Grey parrot. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Animal Behavior Society, Tucson, AZ.Google Scholar
  45. Pepperberg, I. M. (in press-a). Interspecies communication: A tool for assessing conceptual abilities in the African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus). In G. Greenberg & E. Tobach (Eds.),Language, cognition,consciousness: Integrative levels. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  46. Pepperberg, I. M. (in press-b). Evidence for conceptual quantitative abilities in the African Grey parrot: Labeling of cardinal sets.Ethology Google Scholar
  47. Piaget, J. (1952).The origins of intelligence in children (M. Cook, Trans). New York: International Universities Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Piaget, J. (1954)The construction of reality in the child. (M. Cook, Trans). New York: Basic Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Premack, D. (1983). The codes of man and beasts.The Behavioral & Brain Sciences,6, 125–167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Premack, D. (1984). Upgrading a mind. In T. G. Bever, J. M. Carroll, & L. A. Miller (Eds.),Talking minds. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  51. Redshaw, M. (1978). Cognitive development in human and gorilla infantsJournal of Human Evolution,7, 133–141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Rice, M. (1980).Cognition to language. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
  53. Rubinstein, R. A. (1979). The cognitive consequences of bilingual education in northern Belize.American Ethnologist,6 583–601.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Schusterman, R., &Krieger, K. (1984). California sea lions are capable of semantic comprehension.Psychological Record,34, 3–23.Google Scholar
  55. Steckol, K. F., &Leonard, L. B. (1981). Sensonmotor development and the use of prelinguistic performatives.Journal of Speech & Hearing Research,24, 262–268.Google Scholar
  56. Steklis, H. D., &Raleigh, M. H. (1979). Requisites for language: Interspecific and evolutionary aspects. In H. D. Steklis & M. H. Raleigh (Eds.),Neurobiology of social communication in primates. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  57. Stettner, L. J., &Matyniak, K. (1968). The brain of birds.Scientific American,218, 64–76.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Triana, E., &Pasnak, R. (1981). Object permanence in cats and dogs.Animal Learning & Behavior,9, 135–139.Google Scholar
  59. Uzgiris, I. C., &Hunt, J. McV. (1966).Ordinal scales of infant development, No. 1 — Object permanence [Film], Champaign: University of Illinois Films.Google Scholar
  60. Uzgiris, I. C., &Hunt, J. McV. (1975).Assessment in infancy: Ordinal scales of psychological development. Champaign-Urbana: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
  61. Vander Wall, S. B., &Balda, R. P. (1977) Coadaptations of the Clark’s nutcracker and the pinyon pine for efficient seed harvest and dispersal.Ecological Monographs,47, 89–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Vander Wal, S. B., &Balda, R. P. (1981). Ecology and evolution of food-storage behavior in conifer-seed-caching corvidsZeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie,56, 217–242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Vaughter, R. M., Smotherman, W., &Ordy, J. M. (1972). Development of object permanence in the infant squirrel monkeyDevelopmental Psychology,7, 34–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Wise, K. L., Wise, L. A., &Zimmermann, R. R. (1974). Piagetian object permanence in the infant rhesus monkeyDevelopmental Psychology,10, 429–437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Wood, S., Moriarty, K. M., Gardner, B. T., &Gardner, R. A. (1980) Object permanence in child and chimpanzee.Animal Learning & Behavior,8, 3–9.Google Scholar
  66. Zorina, Z. A. (1982). Reasoning ability and adaptivity of behavior in birds. In V. J. A. Novák & J. Mlikovsky (Eds),Evolution and environment. Praha: ČSÁV.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 1986

Authors and Affiliations

  • Irene M. Pepperberg
    • 1
  • Florence A. Kozak
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Anthropology and Program on Language and CognitionNorthwestern UniversityEvanston

Personalised recommendations