Advertisement

Memory & Cognition

, Volume 13, Issue 6, pp 529–537 | Cite as

Surface form and the spacing effect

  • Denise Dellarosa
  • Lyle E. Bourne
Article

Abstract

Deficient processing theories of the spacing effect attribute poor recall of massed-repeated items to a failure to process one or both of the presentations fully. An implication of this approach is that anything that increases the probability that a repetition will receive full processing, or conversely, anything that decreases the probability that the item will be recognized as a repetition, should improve memorability of the item. The present set of experiments tested this prediction by manipulating the surface structure of repeated sentences. On the basis of previous research, it was assumed that memory for surface structure of sentences decays rapidly, and hence can contribute to initial identification of repetitions only at short spacings. Because this manipulation should hinder recognition of repetitions as repetitions, it was expected to induce full processing of massed repetitions, and thus facilitate recall of these items. This prediction was supported. When sentences were repeated verbatim (Experiment 1) or by the same speaker (Experiment 2), the typical spacing effect was obtained. However, when the surface structure or speaker changed at time of repetition, massed repetitions were recalled nearly as well (Experiment 1) or as well (Experiment 2) as their spaced counterparts.

Keywords

Free Recall Verbal Learn Verbal Behavior Spacing Effect Repetition Type 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Anderson, J. R., &Paulson, R. (1977). Representation and retention of verbatim informationJournal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,16, 439–451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Begg, I. (1971). Recognition memory for sentence meaning and wording,Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,10, 176–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Cornell, E. H. (1980). Distributed study facilitates infants’ delayed recognition memory.Memory & Cognition,8, 539–542Google Scholar
  4. Cuddy, L. J., &Jacoby, L. L. (1982). When forgetting helps memory: An analysis of repetition effectsJournal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,21, 451–467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. D’Agostino, P. R., &Deremer, P. (1973). Repetition effects as a function of rehearsal and encoding variability.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,12, 108–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Elmes, D. G., &Bjork, R. A. (1975). The interaction of encoding and rehearsal processes in the recall of repeated and non-repeated words.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,14, 30–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Garrod, S., &Trabasso, T. (1973). A dual-memory information processing interpretation of sentence comprehensionJournal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,12, 155–167CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Gartman, L. M., &Johnson, N. F. (1972). Massed vs. distributed repetitions of homographs A test of the differential encoding hypothesis.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,11, 801–808.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Geiselman, R. E., &Bellezza, F. S. (1976). Long-term memory for speaker’s voice and source location.Memory & Cognition,4, 483–489Google Scholar
  10. Geiselman, R. E., &Bellezza, F. S. (1977). Incidental retention of speaker’s voice.Memory, & Cognition,5, 658–665Google Scholar
  11. Geiselman, R. E., &Crawley, J. M. (1983). Incidental processing of speaker characteristics Voice as connotative information.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,22, 15–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Glanzer, M., &Duarte, A. (1971). Repetition between and within languages in free recallJournal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,10, 625–630.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Glenberg, A. M. (1979). Component-levels theory of the effects of spacing of repetitions on recall and recognition,Memory & Cognition,7, 95–112Google Scholar
  14. Glenberg, A. M., &Smith, S. M. (1981). Spacing repetitions and solving problems are not the same.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,20, 110–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Hintzman, D. L. (1974). Theoretical implications of the spacing effect In R. L. Solso (Ed.),Theories in cognitive psychology: The Loyola Symposium, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  16. Hintzman, D. L., Block, R. A., &Summers, J. J. (1973). Modality tags and memory for repetitions: Laws of spacing effect.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,12, 229–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Hintzman, D. L., Summers, J. J., Eki, N. T., &Moore, M. D. (1975). Voluntary attention and the spacing effect.Memory & Cognition,3, 576–580.Google Scholar
  18. Jacoby, L. L. (1978). On interpreting the effects of repetition: Solving a problem versus remembering a solution.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,17, 649–667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Jarvella, R. J. (1971). Syntactic processing of connected speech.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,10, 409–416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Jarvella, R. J. (1973). Coreference and short-term memory for discourse.Journal of Experimental Psychology,98, 426–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Kahneman, D., &Beatty, J. (1966). Pupil diameter and load on memory,Science,154, 1583–1585.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Keenan, J. M., Macwhinney, B., &Mahew, D. (1977). Pragmatics in memory: A study of natural conversation.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,16, 549–560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kintsch, W. (1975). Memory representation of text. In R. L. Solso, (Ed.),Information processing and cognition, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  24. Kintsch, W., &Bates, E. (1977). Recognition memory for statements from a classroom lecture.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,3, 150–159.Google Scholar
  25. Macfarland, C. E., Rhodes, D. D., &Frey, T. J. (1979). Semantic feature variability and spacing effect.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,18, 163–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Madigan, S. A. (1969). An intraserial repetition and coding processes in free recall.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,8, 828–835CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Magliero, A. (1983). Pupil dilations following pairs of identical words and related to-be-remembered words.Memory & Cognition,11, 609–615.Google Scholar
  28. Postman, L., &Knecht, K. (1983). Encoding variability and retention.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,22, 133–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Rose, R. J. (1980). Encoding variability, levels of processing, and the effects of spacing upon judgments of frequency.Memory & Cognition,18, 84–93.Google Scholar
  30. Rose, R. J., &Rowe, E. J. (1976). Effects of orienting task, spacing of repetitions, and list context on judgments of frequency.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory,2, 142–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Ross, B. H., &Landauer, T. K. (1978). Memory for at least one of two items: Test and failure of retrieval theories of spacing effects.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,17, 669–680.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Rothkopf, E. Z., &Coke, E. U. (1963). Repetition internal and rehearsal method in learning equivalences from written sentences.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,2, 406–416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Rundus, D. (1971). Analysis of rehearsal processes in free recall.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,89, 63–77.Google Scholar
  34. Sachs, J. S. (1967). Recognition memory for syntactic and semantic aspects of connected discourse.Perception & Psychophysics,2, 437–442.Google Scholar
  35. Shaughnessy, J. J., Zimmerman, J., &Underwood, B. J. (1972) Further evidence on the MP-DP effect in free recall learningJournal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,11, 1–12CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Silverstein, L. D. (1977).Repetition and distribution effects on memory: A psychophysiological analysis. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Florida, Gainesville.Google Scholar
  37. Slamecka, N. J., &Barlow, W. (1979). The role of semantic and surface features in word repetition effectsJournal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,18, 617–627CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Toppino, T. C., &Digeorge, W. (1984). The spacing effect in free recall emerges with developmentMemory & Cognition,12, 118–122Google Scholar
  39. Underwood, B. J. (1970). A breakdown of the total-time law in free recall learning.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,9, 573–580.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Waugh, N. C. (1970). On the effective duration of a repeated word.Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior,19, 587–595CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Wright, P. (1969) Transformations and the understanding of sentencesLanguage & Speech, 156-166.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 1985

Authors and Affiliations

  • Denise Dellarosa
    • 1
  • Lyle E. Bourne
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of ColoradoBoulder

Personalised recommendations