Loss of controllability in appetitive situations interferes with subsequent learning in aversive situations
- 230 Downloads
- 1 Citations
Abstract
In two experiments, we examined whether or not a loss of control over food availability would interfere with subsequent two-way shuttle-escape learning. Rats that had experienced loss of control over food delivery were impaired in their acquisition of a shuttle-escape response, relative to the response-contingent and the continuous-reinforcement control rats (in Experiments 1 and 2) and to the lack-of-control and home cage control rats (in Experiment 2). Rats that had received noncontingent food delivery without a prior history of control over food exhibited poorer performance than did the home cage control rats. Moreover, loss of control resulted in a larger interference effect than did lack of control, supporting the view that the learning of response-outcome noncontingency is the main determinant of the interference effect.
Keywords
Interference Effect Animal Behavior Process Target Response Inescapable Shock Horne CageReferences
- Anisman, H. (1975). Time-dependent variations in aversively motivated behaviors: Nonassociative effects of cholinergic and catecholaminergic activity.Psychological Review,82, 359–385.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Beatty, W. W., &Maki, W. S. (1979). Acquisition of instrumental responding following noncontingent reinforcement: Failure to observe “learned laziness” in rats.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society,13, 268–271.Google Scholar
- Calef, R. S., Choban, M. C., Dickson, M. W., Newman, P. D., Boyle, M., Baxa, N. D., &Geller, E. S. (1989). The effects of noncontingent reinforcement on the behavior of a previously learned running response.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society,27, 263–266.Google Scholar
- Calef, R. S., Metz, R. A., Atkinson, T. L., Pellerzi, R. C, Taylor, K. S., &Geller, E. S. (1984). Acquisition of running in the straight alley following experience with response-independent food.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society,22, 67–69.Google Scholar
- Calef, R. S., Murray, D. W., Modlin, P. D., Meekins, B. W., &Geller, E. S. (1981). The effects of controllability on extinction.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society,17, 241–243.Google Scholar
- Caspy, T., &Lubow, R. E. (1981). Generality of US preexposure effects: Transfer from food to shock or shock to food with and without the same response requirements.Animal Learning & Behavior,9, 524–532.Google Scholar
- Dess, N. K., Chapman, C. D., &Minor, T. R. (1988). Inescapable shock increases finickiness about drinking quinine-adulterated water in rats.Learning & Motivation,19, 408–424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Glazer, H. I., &Weiss, J. M. (1976a). Long-term and transitory interference effect.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes,2, 191–201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Glazer, H. I., &Weiss, J. M. (1976b). Long-term interference effect: An alternative to “learned helplessness”.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes,2, 202–213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Goodkin, F. (1976). Rats learn the relationship between responding and environmental events: An expansion of the learned helplessness hypothesis.Learning & Motivation,7, 382–393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hanson, J. D., Larson, M. E., &Snowdon, C. T. (1976). The effects of control over high intensity noise on plasma Cortisol levels in rhesus monkeys.Behavioral Biology,16, 333–340.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Jackson, R. L., &Minor, T. R. (1988). Effects of signaling inescapable shock on subsequent escape learning: Implications for theories of coping and “learned helplessness”.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes,14, 390–400.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Job, R. F. S. (1987). Learned helplessness in an appetitive discretetrial T-maze discrimination test.Animal Learning & Behavior,15, 342–346.Google Scholar
- Job, R. F. S. (1988). Interference and facilitation produced by noncontingent reinforcement in the appetitive situation.Animal Learning & Behavior,16, 451–460.Google Scholar
- Job, R. F. S. (1989). A test of proposed mechanisms underlying the interference effect produced by noncontingent food presentation.Learning & Motivation,20, 153–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Levis, D. J. (1976). Learned helplessness: A reply and an alternative S-R interpretation.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,105, 47–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Maier, S. F., &Seligman, M. E. P. (1976). Learned helplessness: Theory and evidence.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,105, 3–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Maier, S. F., Seligman, M. E. P., &Solomon, R. L. (1969). Pavlovian fear conditioning and learned helplessness: Effects on escape and avoidance behavior of (a) the CS-US contingency and (b) the independence of the US and voluntary responding. In B. A. Campbell & R. M. Church (Eds.),Punishment and aversive behavior (pp. 299–342). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
- Minor, T. R., &LoLordo, V. M. (1984). Escape deficits following inescapable shock: The role of contextual odor.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes,10, 168–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Mullins, G. P., &Winefield, A. H. (1977). Immunization and helplessness phenomena in the rat in a nonaversive situation.Animal Learning & Behavior,5, 281–284.Google Scholar
- Oakes, W. F., Rosenblum, J.L., &Fox, P. E. (1982). “Manna from heaven”: The effect of noncontingent appetitive reinforcers on learning in rats.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society,19, 123–126.Google Scholar
- Overmier, J. B., &Seligman, M. E. P. (1967). Effects of inescapable shock upon subsequent escape and avoidance responding.Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology,63, 28–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Plonsky, M., Warren, D. A., &Rosellini, R. A. (1984). The effects of inescapable shock on appetitive motivation.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society,22, 229–231.Google Scholar
- Rosellini, R. A. (1978). Inescapable shock interferes with the acquisition of an appetitive operant.Animal Learning & Behavior,6, 155–159.Google Scholar
- Rosellini, R. A., &DeCola, J. P. (1981). Inescapable shock interferes with the acquisition of a low-activity response in an appetitive context.Animal Learning & Behavior,9, 487–490.Google Scholar
- Rosellini, R. A., DeCola, J. P., &Shapiro, N. R. (1982). Crossmotivational effects of inescapable shock are associative in nature.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes,8, 376–388.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Seligman, M. E. P. (1975).Helplessness: On depression, development, and death. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
- Seligman, M. E. P., &Maier, S. F. (1967). Failure to escape traumatic shock.Journal of Experimental Psychology,74, 1–9.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Seligman, M. E. P., Rosellini, R. A., &Kozak, M. J. (1975). Learned helplessness in the rat: Time course, immunization, and reversibility.Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology,88, 542–547.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Sonoda, A. (1990). Effect of uncontrollable shock on subsequent appetitive discrimination learning in rats.Japanese Journal of Psychonomic Science,8, 95–100. (Japanese, with English abstract)Google Scholar
- Tsuda, A., Ida, Y., &Tanaka, M. (1989). Differential effects of uncontrollability versus loss of stressor controllability on rat brain noradrenaline metabolism.Neurosciences,15, 253–255. (Japanese, with English abstract)Google Scholar
- Tsuda, A., Tanaka, M., Nishikawa, T., &Hirai, H. (1983). Effects of coping behavior on gastric lesions in rats as a function of the complexity of coping tasks.Physiology & Behavior,30, 805–808.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Warren, D. A., Rosellini, R. A., Plonsky, M., &DeCola, J. P. (1985). Learned helplessness and immunization: Sensitivity to response-reinforcer independence in immunized rats.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes,11, 576–590.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Weiss, J. M. (1971). Effects of punishing the coping response (conflict) on stress pathology in rats.Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology,77, 14–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Weiss, J. M., Goodman, P. A., Losito, B. G., Corrigan, S., Charry, J. M, &Bailey, W. H. (1981). Behavioral depression produced by an uncontrollable stressor: Relationship to norepinephrine, dopamine, and serotonin levels in various regions of rat brain.Brain Research Reviews,3, 167–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Wight, M. T., &Katzev, R. D. (1977). Noncontingent positive reinforcers retard later escape/avoidance learning in rats.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society,9, 319–321.Google Scholar
- Williams, J. L., &Maier, S. F. (1977). Transituational immunization and therapy of learned helplessness in the rat.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes,3, 240–252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar