Skip to main content

Advertisement

SpringerLink
  • Log in
  1. Home
  2. Memory & Cognition
  3. Article
The role of bigram frequency in the perception of words and nonwords
Download PDF
Your article has downloaded

Similar articles being viewed by others

Slider with three articles shown per slide. Use the Previous and Next buttons to navigate the slides or the slide controller buttons at the end to navigate through each slide.

Chinese lexical database (CLD)

22 June 2018

Ching Chu Sun, Peter Hendrix, … Rolf Harald Baayen

On non-adjacent letter repetition and orthographic processing: Lexical decisions to nonwords created by repeating or inserting letters in words

24 November 2020

Emilia Kerr, Jonathan Mirault & Jonathan Grainger

How much time does it take to discriminate two sets by their numbers of elements?

28 April 2022

Jüri Allik & Aire Raidvee

Judging Numbers: Global and Local Contextual Effects in Individual and Group Data

16 April 2021

Francisco J. Silva, Paulina N. Silva & Kathleen M. Silva

Multi-LEX: A database of multi-word frequencies for French and English

28 November 2022

Marjorie Armando, Jonathan Grainger & Stephane Dufau

The Glasgow Norms: Ratings of 5,500 words on nine scales

11 September 2018

Graham G. Scott, Anne Keitel, … Sara C. Sereno

Shabd: A psycholinguistic database for Hindi

06 August 2021

Ark Verma, Vivek Sikarwar, … Pawan Kumar

Psycholinguistic measures for German verb pairs: Semantic transparency, semantic relatedness, verb family size, and age of reading acquisition

18 June 2018

Eva Smolka & Carsten Eulitz

Effects of lexical ambiguity, frequency, and acoustic details in auditory perception

15 October 2018

Chelsea Sanker

Download PDF
  • Published: September 1975

The role of bigram frequency in the perception of words and nonwords

  • Glenn A. Rice1 &
  • David Owen Robinson2 

Memory & Cognition volume 3, pages 513–518 (1975)Cite this article

  • 1882 Accesses

  • 38 Citations

  • Metrics details

Abstract

Adult subjects decided whether strings of four, five, or six letters formed words or nonwords. Words and nonwords were equally probable stimuli, and there were equal numbers of stimuli of high and low mean bigram frequency. Half the words were common and half occurred rarely i[~ printed English. Mean bigram frequency had no effect on subjects’ response times for common words and for nonwords, but the reaction times for rare words were significantly longer for words of high mean bigram frequency than for words of low mean bigram frequency. This result may be accounted for by a modification of Rumelhart and Siple’s (1974) model of word perception to include the mapping of simple features to units which correspond to common spelling patterns.

Download to read the full article text

Working on a manuscript?

Avoid the common mistakes

References

  • Baron, J., & Thurston, I. An analysis of the word-superiority effect. Cognitive Psychology, 1973,4, 207–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biederman, G. B. The recognition of tachistoscopically presented five-letter words as a function of digram frequency. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1966,5, 208–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E., & Gregory, M. Visual perception of words differing in letter digrarm frequency. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1968,7, 569–571.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, N. B., Davies, P., & Richman, B.The American heritage word frequency book. Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1971.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H. The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1973,12, 335–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J., Osser, H., & Pick, A. D. A study of the development of grapheme-phoneme correspondences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1963,2, 142–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howes, D. H., & Solomon, R. L. Visual threshold as a function of word-probability. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1951,41, 401–410.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kucera, H., & Francis, W. N.Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence: Brown University Press, 1967.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landauer, T. K., & Streeter, L. A. Structural differences between common and rare words: Failure of equivalence assumptions for theories of word recognition. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1973,12, 119–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayzner, M. S., & Tresselt, M. E. Tables of single-letter and digram frequency counts for various word-length and letter-position combinations. Psychonomic Monograph Supplements, 1965,1, 13–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A., Brunet, J. S., & Postman, L. Familiarity of letter sequences and tachistoseopic identfication. Journal of General Psychology, 1954,50, 129–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Millward, R. B., Aikin, J. O., & Wickens, T. D. The Human Learning Laboratory at Brown University. InComputers in the psychological laboratory, Vol. 2. Maynard, Mass.: Digital Equipment Corporation, 1972.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U.Cognitive psychology. New York; Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1967.

    Google Scholar 

  • Owsowitz, S. E. The effects of word familiarity and letter structure familiarity on the perception of words. Rand Corporation Publications, No. P-2820, 1963.

  • Reicher, G. M. Perceptual recognition as a function of meaningfulness of stimulus material. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1969,81, 274–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubenstein, H., Garfield, L., & Millikan, J. Homographic entries in the internal lexicon. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1970,9, 487–492.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubenstein, H., Lewis, S. S., & Rubenstein, M. A. Homographic entries in the internal lexicon: Effects of systematicity and relative frequency of meanings. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1971,10, 57–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart, D. E., & Siple, P. The process of recognizing tachistoscopically presented words. Psychological Review, 1974,81, 99–118.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. E., & Spoehr, K. T. The perception of printed English: A theoretical perspective, In B. H. Kantowitz (Ed.),Human information processing: Tutorials in performance and cognition. Potomac, Md.: Erlbaum Press, 1974.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, F.Understanding reading. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1971.

    Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, R. L., & Howes, D. H. Word-probability, personal values, and visual duration thresholds. Psychological Review, 1951,58, 256–370.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trabasso, T., Rollins, H. & Shaughnessy, E. Storage and verification stages in processing concepts. Cognitive Psychology, 1971,2, 239–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler, D. D. Processes in word recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 1970,1, 59–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winer, B. J.Statistical principles in experimental design. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1962.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

  1. Nichols College, 01570, Dudley, Massachusetts

    Glenn A. Rice

  2. Brown University, 02912, Providence, Rhode Island

    David Owen Robinson

Authors
  1. Glenn A. Rice
    View author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

  2. David Owen Robinson
    View author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

Additional information

This research was supported in part by Grant No. GB-34122 from the National Science Foundation to Dr. Richard B. Millward, R. B. Millward.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Rice, G.A., Robinson, D.O. The role of bigram frequency in the perception of words and nonwords. Memory & Cognition 3, 513–518 (1975). https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197523

Download citation

  • Received: 26 December 1974

  • Accepted: 07 February 1975

  • Issue Date: September 1975

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197523

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

Keywords

  • Word Frequency
  • Reaction Time Task
  • Common Word
  • Paradoxical Effect
  • Bigram Frequency
Download PDF

Working on a manuscript?

Avoid the common mistakes

Advertisement

Over 10 million scientific documents at your fingertips

Switch Edition
  • Academic Edition
  • Corporate Edition
  • Home
  • Impressum
  • Legal information
  • Privacy statement
  • California Privacy Statement
  • How we use cookies
  • Manage cookies/Do not sell my data
  • Accessibility
  • FAQ
  • Contact us
  • Affiliate program

Not logged in - 44.200.171.156

Not affiliated

Springer Nature

© 2023 Springer Nature Switzerland AG. Part of Springer Nature.