Advertisement

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

, Volume 11, Issue 6, pp 1099–1104 | Cite as

Causal models frame interpretation of mathematical equations

  • Daniel Mochon
  • Steven A. SlomanEmail author
Brief Reports
  • 289 Downloads

Abstract

We offer evidence that people can construe mathematical relations as causal. The studies show that people can select the causal versions of equations and that their selections predict both what they consider most understandable and how they expect variables to influence one another. When asked to write down equations, people have a strong preference for the version that matches their causal model. Causal models serve to structure equations by determining the preferred order of variables: Causes should be on one side of an equality, and a single effect should appear on the other.

Keywords

Causal Model Causal Structure Modal Choice Causal Interpretation Causal Graph 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Andersson, B. (1986). The experiential gestalt of causation: A common core to pupil’s preconceptions in science.European Journal of Science Education,8, 155–171.Google Scholar
  2. Chi, M. T. H., Feltovich, P. J., &Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices.Cognitive Science,5, 121–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. DiSessa, A. A. (1993). Towards an epistemology of physics.Cognition & Instruction,10, 105–225.Google Scholar
  4. Driver, R., Guesne, E., &Tiberghien, A. (1993). Some features of children’s ideas and their implications for teaching. In R. Driver, E. Guesne, & A. Tiberghien (Eds.),Children’s ideas in science (pp. 193–201). Buckingham, U.K.: Open University.Google Scholar
  5. Glymour, C. (2001).The mind’s arrows: Bayes nets and graphical causal models in psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  6. Hall, R., Kibler, D., Wenger, E., &Truxaw, C. (1989). Exploring the episodic structure of algebra story problem solving.Cognition & Instruction,6, 223–283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Hinsley, D. A., Hayes, J. R., &Simon, H. A. (1977). From words to equations: Meaning and representation in algebra word problems. In M. A. Just & P. A. Carpenter (Eds.),Cognitive processes in comprehension (pp. 89–105). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  8. Hunt, E., &Minstrell, J. (1994). A cognitive approach to the teaching of physics. In K. McGilly (Ed.),Classroom lessons: Integrating cognitive theory and classroom practice (pp. 51–74). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  9. Larkin, J. (1983). The role of problem representation in physics. In D. Gentner & A. Stevens (Eds.),Mental models (pp. 75–98). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  10. Nathan, M. J., Kintsch, W., &Young, E. (1992). A theory of algebra-word-problem comprehension and its implications for the design of learning environments.Cognition & Instruction,9, 329–389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Paige, J. M., &Simon, H. A. (1966). Cognitive processes in solving word problems. In B. Kleinmuntz (Ed.),Problem solving: Research, method, and theory (pp. 51–119). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  12. Pearl, J. (2000).Causality: Models, reasoning and inference. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  13. Priest, A. G., &Lindsay, R. O. (1992). New light on novice-expert differences in physics problem solving.British Journal of Psychology,83, 389–405.Google Scholar
  14. Reif, F., &Allen, S. (1992). Cognition for interpreting scientific concepts: A study of acceleration.Cognition & Instruction,9, 1–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Reiner, M., Slotta, J. D., Chi, M. T. H., &Resnick, L. B. (2000). Naive physics reasoning: A commitment to substance-based conceptions.Cognition & Instruction,18, 1–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Sherin, B. L. (2001). How students understand physics equations.Cognition & Instruction,19, 479–541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Sloman, S. [A.], &Lagnado, D. [A.] (2004). Causal invariance in reasoning and learning. In B. Ross (Ed.),The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 44, pp. 287–325). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  18. Sloman, S. A., &Lagnado, D. A. (2005). Do we “do”?Cognitive Science,29, 5–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Spirtes, P., Glymour, C., &Scheines, R. (1993).Causation, prediction, and search. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  20. Waldmann, M. R. (1996). Knowledge-based causal induction. In D. R. Shanks, K. J. Holyoak, & D. L. Medin (Eds.),The psychology of learning and motivation: Vol. 34. Causal learning (pp. 47–88). San Diego: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. White, B. Y. (1993). ThinkerTools: Causal models, conceptual change, and science education.Cognition & Instruction,10, 1–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2004

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Cognitive and Linguistic SciencesBrown UniversityProvidence

Personalised recommendations