The cognitive neuroscience of creativity

Abstract

This article outlines a framework of creativity based on functional neuroanatomy. Recent advances in the field of cognitive neuroscience have identified distinct brain circuits that are involved in specific higher brain functions. To date, these findings have not been applied to research on creativity. It is proposed that there are four basic types of creative insights, each mediated by a distinctive neural circuit. By definition, creative insights occur in consciousness. Given the view that the working memory buffer of the prefrontal cortex holds the content of consciousness, each of the four distinctive neural loops terminates there. When creativity is the result of deliberate control, as opposed to spontaneous generation, the prefrontal cortex also instigates the creative process. Both processing modes, deliberate and spontaneous, can guide neural computation in structures that contribute emotional content and in structures that provide cognitive analysis, yielding the four basic types of creativity. Supportive evidence from psychological, cognitive, and neuroscientific studies is presented and integrated in this article. The new theoretical framework systematizes the interaction between knowledge and creative thinking, and how the nature of this relationship changes as a function of domain and age. Implications for the arts and sciences are briefly discussed.

References

  1. Amabile, T. (1983).The social psychology of creativity. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Anderson, J. R., &Bower, G. H. (1973).Human associative memory. Washington, DC: Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Ashby, G. F., Isen, A. M., &Turken, A. U. (1999). A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition.Psychological Review,106, 529–550.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Ashby, G. F., Valentin, V. V., &Turken, A. U. (2002). The effects of positive affect and arousal on working memory and executive attention: Neurobiology and computational models. In S. Moore & M. Oaksford (Eds.),Emotional cognition: From brain to behaviour (pp. 245–287). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Axelrod, B. N., Jiron, C. C., &Henry, R. R. (1993). Performance of adults ages 20 to 90 on the abbreviated Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.Clinical Neuropsychology,7, 205–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Baars, B. J. (1989).A cognitive theory of consciousness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Baddeley, A. (1996). Exploring the central executive.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,49A, 5–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory.Trends in Cognitive Sciences,4, 417–423.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Baker-Sennett, J., &Ceci, S. (1996). Clue-efficiency and insight: Unveiling the mystery of inductive leaps.Journal of Creative Behavior,30, 153–172.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Bekhtereva, N. P., Dan’ko, S. G., Starchenko, M. G., Pakhomov, S. V., &Medvedev, S. V. (2001). Study of the brain organization of creativity: III. Positron-emission tomography data.Human Physiology,27, 390–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Bekhtereva, N. P., Starchenko, M. G., Klyucharev, V. A., Vorob’ev, V. A., Pakhomov, S. V., &Medvedev, S. V. (2000). Study of the brain organization of creativity: II. Positron-emission tomography data.Human Physiology,26, 516–522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Boden, J. E., &Boden, G. M. (1969). The other side of the brain III: The corpus callosum and creativity.Bulletin of the Los Angeles Neurological Society,34, 191–203.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Boden, M. A. (1998). Creativity and artificial intelligence.Artificial Intelligence,103, 347–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Boone, B. K. (1999). Neuropsychological assessment of executive functions. In B. L. Miller & J. L. Cummings (Eds.),The human frontal lobes: Functions and disorders (pp. 247–260). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Bransford, J. D., &Stein, B. S. (1984).The ideal problem solver. New York: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Braun, A. R., Balkin, T. J., Wesensten, N. J., Gwadry, F., Carson, R. E., Varga, M., Baldwin, P., Selbie, S., Belenky, G., &Herscovitch, P. (1997). Regional cerebral blood flow throughout the sleep-wake cycle.Brain, 120, 1173–1197.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Broadbent, D. A. (1958).Perception and communication. New York: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Bruckner, R. L. (1996). Beyond HERA: Contributions of specific prefrontal brain areas to long-term memory retrieval.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,3, 149–158.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Cabeza, R., &Nyberg, L. (2000). Imaging cognition II: An empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,12, 1–47.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Cadoret, G., Pike, G. B., &Petrides, M. (2001). Selective activation of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in the human brain during active retrieval processing.European Journal of Neuroscience,14, 1164–1170.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Carlsson, I., Wendt, P. E., &Risberg, J. (2000). On the neurobiology of creativity: Differences in frontal activity between high and low creative subjects.Neuropsychologia,38, 873–885.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Channon, S., &Crawford, S. (1999). Problem-solving in real-lifetype situations: The effect of anterior and posterior lesions on performance.Neuropsychologia,37, 757–770.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Churchland, P. S. (2002).Brain-wise. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading activation theory of semantic processing.Psychological Review,82, 407–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Conway, A. A. R., Cowan, N., &Bunting, M. F. (2001). The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: The importance of working memory capacity.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,8, 331–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Courtney, S. M., Petit, L., Haxby, J. V., &Ungerleider, L. G. (1998). The role of prefrontal cortex in working memory: Examining the contents of consciousness.Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B,353, 1819–1828.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Cowan, N. (1995).Attention and memory: An integrated framework. Oxford Psychology Series, No. 26. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Cowan, N. (2001). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity.Behavioral & Brain Sciences,24, 87–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Crick, F. H. C., &Koch, C. (1998). Consciousness and neuroscience.Cerebral Cortex,8, 97–107.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Society, culture, person: A systems view of creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.),The nature of creativity (pp. 325–339). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Damasio, A. R. (1994).Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Putnam.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Damasio, A. R. (2001). Some notes on brain, imagination and creativity. In K. H. Pfenninger & V. R. Shubik (Eds.),The origins of creativity (pp. 59–68). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Damasio, A. R. (2002).Conference proceedings: Neuroethics. Mapping the field. New York: Dana.

    Google Scholar 

  34. DeBono, E. (1968).New think: The use of lateral thinking in the generation of new ideas. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Dehaene, S., &Naccache, L. (2001). Toward a cognitive science of consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework.Cognition,79, 1–37.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Desgranges, B., Baron, J.-C., &Eustache, F. (1997). The functional neuroanatomy of episodic memory: The role of the frontal lobes, the hippocampal formation, and other areas.NeuroImage,8, 198–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Dietrich, A. (2003). Functional neuroanatomy of altered states of consciousness. The transient hypofrontality hypothesis.Consciousness & Cognition,12, 231–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Dietrich, A. (2004). Neurocognitive mechanisms underlying the experience of flow.Consciousness & Cognition,13, 746–761.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Dietrich, A., &Allen, J. D. (1998). Functional dissociation of the prefrontal cortex and the hippocampus in timing behavior.Behavioral Neuroscience,112, 1043–1047.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Dietrich, A., Taylor, J. T., &Passmore, C. E. (2001). AVP (4-8) improves concept learning in PFC-damaged but not hippocampal-damaged rats.Brain Research,919, 41–47.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Duncan, J., &Owen, A. M. (2000). Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands.Trends in Neurosciences,23, 475–483.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Duncker, K. (1945). On problem solving.Psychological Monographs,585, (No 270).

  43. Eysenck, H. J. (1993). Creativity and personality: Suggestions for a theory.Psychological Inquiry,4, 147–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Eysenck, H. J. (1995).Genius: The natural history of creativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Fink, G. R., Markowitsch, H. J., Reinkemeier, M., Bruckbauer, T., Kessler, J., &Heiss, W. D. (1996). Cerebral representation of one’s own past: Neural networks involved in autobiographical memory.Journal of Neuroscience,16, 4275–4282.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Finke, R. A. (1996). Imagery, creativity, and emergent structure.Consciousness & Cognition,5, 381–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Fletcher, P. C., Anderson, J. M., Shanks, D. R., Honey, R., Carpenter, T. A., Donovan, T., Papdakis, N., &Bullmore, E. T. (2001). Responses of the human frontal cortex to surprising events are predicted by formal associative learning theory.Nature Neuroscience,4, 1043–1048.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Frensch, P. A., &Sternberg, R. J. (1989). Expertise and intelligent thinking: When is it worse to know better? In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.),Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 5, pp. 157–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Friedman, R. S., &Förster, J. (2002). The influence of approach and avoidance motor actions on creative cognition.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,38, 41–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Frith, C. D., &Dolan, R. (1996). The role of the prefrontal cortex in higher cognitive functions.Cognitive Brain Research,5, 175–181.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Frith, C. D., &Frith, U. (2001). Cognitive psychology-Interacting minds-A biological basis.Science,286, 1692–1695.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Fuster, J. M. (1995). Temporal processing-Structure and function of the human prefrontal cortex.Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 769, 173–181.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Fuster, J. M. (2000a). Executive frontal functions.Experimental Brain Research,133, 66–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Fuster, J. M. (2000b). The prefrontal cortex of the primate: A synopsis.Psychobiology,28, 125–131.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Fuster, J. M. (2002). Frontal lobe and cognitive development.Journal of Neurocytology,31, 373–385.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Gabrieli, J. D. E. (1998). Cognitive neuroscience of human memory.Annual Review of Psychology,49, 87–115.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Gardner, H. (1993).Creating minds: An anatomy of creativity seen though the lives of Freud, Einstein, Picasso, Stravinsky, Eliot, Graham, and Gandhi. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Gazzaniga, S. M., Ivry, R. B., &Mangun, G. R. (1998).Cognitive neuroscience. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Gilbert, P. F. C. (2001). An outline of brain function.Cognitive Brain Research,12, 61–74.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1992). Working memory and the mind.Scientific American,267, 111–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Goodwin, D. W. (1992). Alcohol as muse.American Journal of Psychotherapy,46, 422–433.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Gruber, H. E. (1981).Darwin on man: A psychological study of scientific creativity. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  63. Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity.American Psychologist,5, 444–454.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Guilford, J. P. (1967).The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  65. Guilford, J. P. (1982). Is some creative thinking irrational?Journal of Creative Behavior,16, 151–154.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Halford, G. S., Wilson, W. H., &Phillips, S. (1998). Processing capacity defined by relational complexity: Implications for comparative, developmental, and cognitive psychology.Behavioral & Brain Sciences,21, 723–802.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Hartman, E. (1966). The psychophysiology of free will. In R. Lowenstein, L. Newman, & A. Solnit (Eds.),Psychoanalysis: A general psychology. New York: International University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  68. Hasegawa, I., Hayashi, T., &Miyashita, Y. (1999). Memory retrieval under the control of the prefrontal cortex.Annals of Medicine,31, 380–387.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Hayes, R. R. (1989). Cognitive processes in creativity. In J. A. Glover, R. R. Ronning, & C. R. Reynolds (Eds.),Handbook of creativity (pp. 135–145). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  70. Hebb, D. O. (1939). Intelligence in man after large removal of cerebral tissue: Report of four left frontal lobe cases.Journal of General Psychology,21, 73–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Hobson, J. A. (1988).The dreaming brain. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  72. Hobson, J. A., & McCarley, R. (1977). The brain as a dream-state generator: An activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream process.American Journal of Psychiatry,134, 1335–1348.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Hobson, J. A., Pace-Schott, E. F., &Stickhold, R. (2000). Dreaming and the brain: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states.Behavioural & Brain Sciences,23, 793–842.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Hull, D. L., Tessner, P. D., &Diamond, A. M. (1978). Planck’s principle: Do younger scientists accept new scientific ideas with greater alacrity than older scientists?Science,202, 717–723.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Jamison, K. R. (1993).Touched by fire: Manic depressive illness and the artistic temperament. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  76. Janowsky, J. S., Shimamura, A. P., &Squire, L. R. (1989). Source memory impairment in patients with frontal lobe lesions.Neuropsychologia,27, 1043–1056.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Jasper, H. H. (1995). A historical perspective: The rise and fall of prefrontal lobotomy.Advances in Neurology,66, 97–114.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Kane, M. J., Bleckley, M. K., Conway, A. R. A., &Engle, R. W. (2001). A controlled-attention view of working-memory capacity.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,130, 169–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Katz, A. N. (1986). The relationship between creativity and cerebral hemisphericity for creative architects, scientists, and mathematicians.Empirical Studies of the Arts,4, 97–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Keenan, J. P., Wheeler, M. A., Gallup, G. G., &Pascual-Leone, A. (2000). Self-recognition and the right prefrontal cortex.Trends in Cognitive Sciences,4, 338–344.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Kesner, R. P., &Holbrook, T. (1987). Dissociation of item and order spatial memory in rats following medial prefrontal cortex lesions.Neuropsychologia,25, 653–664.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Kikyo, H., Ohki, K., &Sekihara, K. (2001). Temporal characterization of memory retrieval processes: An fMRI study of the “tip of the tongue” phenomenon.European Journal of Neuroscience,14, 887–892.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Kinsbourne, M. (1982). Hemispheric specialization and the growth of human understanding.American Psychologist,37, 411–420.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  84. Knight, R. T., &Grabowecky, M. (1999). Prefrontal cortex, time, and consciousness. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.),The cognitive neurosciences (2nd ed., pp. 1319–1337). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  85. Knoblich, G., Ohlsson, S., Haider, H., &Rhenius, D. (1999). Constraint relaxation and chunk decomposition in insight problem solving.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,25, 1534–1555.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  86. Koechlin, E., Basso, G., Pietrini, P., Panzer, S., &Grafman, J. (1999). The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition.Nature,399, 148–151.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  87. Koestler, A. (1964).The act of creation. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  88. Kolb, B. (1984). Functions of the prefrontal cortex in the rat: A comparative view.Brain Research Review,8, 65–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Kolb, B., &Whishaw, I. Q. (1983). Problems and principles in crossspecies generalizations. In E. T. Robinson (Ed.),Behavioral contribution to brain research (pp. 266–287). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  90. Konishi, S., Nakajima, K., Uchida, I., Kameyama, M., Nakahara, K., Sekihara, K., &Miyashita, Y. (1998). Transient activation of the inferior prefrontal cortex during cognitive set shifting.Nature Neuroscience,1, 80–84.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  91. Kornhuber, H. H. (1993). Prefrontal cortex and homo-sapiens-on creativity and reasoned will.Neurological Psychiatry & Brain Research,2, 1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  92. Kuhn, T. (1970).The structure of scientific revolutions (2nd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  93. LeDoux, J. (1996).The emotional brain. New York: Touchstone.

    Google Scholar 

  94. Lehman, H. C. (1953).Age and achievement. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  95. Lhermitte, F. (1983). “Utilization behaviour” and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes.Brain,106, 237–255.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  96. Lhermitte, F., Pillon, B., &Serdaru, M. (1986). Human autonomy and the frontal lobes: Part I. Imitation and utilization behavior: A neuropsychological study of 75 patients.Annals of Neurology,19, 326–334.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  97. Lunchins, A. S., &Lunchins, E. H. (1959).Rigidity of behavior. Eugene: University of Oregon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  98. Markowitsch, H. J. (1995). Cerebral bases of consciousness: A historical view.Neuropsychologia,33, 1181–1192.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  99. Martindale, C. (1995). Creativity and connectionism. In S. M. Smith, T. B. Ward, & R. A. Finke (Eds.),The creative cognition approach (pp. 249–268). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  100. Martindale, C. (1999). The biological basis of creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.),Handbook of creativity (pp. 137–152). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  101. Mayberg, H. S. (1997). Limbic-cortical dysregulation: A proposed model of depression.Journal of Neuropsychiatry & Clinical Neuroscience,9, 471–481.

    Google Scholar 

  102. Means, L. W., &Holstein, R. D. (1992). Individual aged rats are impaired on repeated reversals due to loss of different behavioral patterns.Physiology & Behavior,52, 959–963.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  103. Mednick, S. A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process.Psychological Review,69, 220–232.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  104. Metcalfe, J. (1986). Feelings of knowing in memory and problem solving.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,12, 288–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  105. Miller, B. L., Cummings, J., Mishkin, F., Boone, K., Prince, F., Ponton, M., &Cotman, C. (1998). Emergence of artistic talent in frontotemporal dementia.Neurology, 51, 978–982.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  106. Miller, E. K., &Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function.Annual Review of Neuroscience,24, 167–202.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  107. Milner, B. (1995). Aspects of human frontal lobe function.Advances in Neurology,66, 67–84.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  108. Mishkin, M., Malamut, B., &Bachevalier, J. (1984). Memory and habit: Two neural systems. In G. Lynch, J. J. McGaugh, & N. M. Weinberger (Eds.),Neurobiology of learning and memory (pp. 66–77). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  109. Monchi, O., Petrides, M., Petre, V., Worsley, K., &Dagher, A. (2001). Wisconsin card sorting revisited: Distinct neural circuits participating in different stages of the task identified by event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging.Journal of Neuroscience,21, 7733–7741.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. Nisbett, R. E., &Ross, L. (1980).Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgments. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  111. Norman, D. A., &Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. S. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.),Consciousness and self-regulation (pp. 1–18). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  112. Nyberg, L. (1998). Mapping episodic memory.Behavioural Brain Research,90, 107–114.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  113. Petrides, M. (1996). Specialized systems for the processing of mnemonic information within the primate frontal cortex.Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B,351, 1455–1462.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  114. Petrides, M., &Pandya, D. N. (1999). Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: Comparative cytoarchitectonic analysis in the human and the macaque brain.European Journal of Neuroscience,11, 1011–1036.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  115. Pfenninger, K. H., &Shubik, V. R. (2001). Insights into the foundation of creativity: A synthesis. In K. H. Pfenninger & V. R. Shubik (Eds.),The origins of creativity (pp. 213–236). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  116. Pinker, S. (1999).How the mind works. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  117. Posner, M. (1994). Attention: The mechanism of consciousness.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,91, 7398–7403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  118. Post, F. (1994). Creativity and psychopathology: A study of 291 worldfamous men.British Journal of Psychiatry,165, 22–34.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  119. Post, F. (1996). Verbal creativity, depression and alcoholism: An investigation of one hundred American and British writers.British Journal of Psychiatry,168, 545–555.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  120. Povinelli, D. J., &Preuss, T. M. (1995). Theory of mind: Evolutionary history of a cognitive specialization.Trends in Neurosciences,18, 418–424.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  121. Quintana, J., &Fuster, J. M. (1999). From perception to action: Temporal integrative function of prefrontal and parietal neurons.Cerebral Cortex,9, 213–221.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  122. Rechtschaffen, A. (1978). The single-mindedness and isolation of dreams.Sleep,1, 97–109.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  123. Rothenberg, A. (2001). Bipolar illness, creativity, and treatment.Psychiatry Quarterly,72, 131–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  124. Rylander, G. (1948). Personality analysis before and after frontal lobotomy.Research Publication—Association for Research in Nervous & Mental Disease,27, 691–705.

    Google Scholar 

  125. Sarter, M., Givens, B., &Bruno, J. P. (2001). The cognitive neuroscience of sustained attention: Where top-down meets bottom-up.Brain Research Reviews,35, 146–160.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  126. Sass, L. A. (2000). Schizophrenia, modernism, and the “creative imagination”: On creativity and psychopathology.Creativity Research Journal,13, 55–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  127. Scheibel, A. B. (1999). Creativity and the brain. Available at http://www.pbs.org/teachersource/scienceline/archives/sept99/sept99.shtm.

  128. Schooler, J. W., &Melcher, J. (1995). The ineffability of insight. In S. M. Smith, T. B. Ward, & R. A. Finke (Eds.),The creative cognition approach (pp. 97–133). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  129. Shallice, T., &Burgess, W. (1991). Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man.Brain,114, 727–741.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  130. Shirley, D. A., &Langan-Fox, J. (1996). Intuition: A review of the literature.Psychological Reports,79, 563–584.

    Google Scholar 

  131. Simonton, D. K. (1975). Creativity, task complexity, and intuitive versus analytical problem solving.Psychological Reports,37, 351–354.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  132. Simonton, D. K. (1988). Creativity, leadership, and chance. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson (Eds.),The nature of creativity: Current psychological perspectives (pp. 386–426). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  133. Simonton, D. K. (1997). Creative productivity: A predictive and explanatory model of career trajectories and landmarks.Psychological Review,104, 66–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  134. Simonton, D. K. (1999). Creativity from a historiometric perspective. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.),Handbook of Creativity (pp. 116–133). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  135. Simonton, D. K. (2000). Creativity: Cognitive, personal, developmental, and social aspects.American Psychologist,55, 151–158.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  136. Simonton, D. K. (2003). Scientific creativity as constrained stochastic behavior: The integration of product, person, and process.Psychological Bulletin,129, 475–494.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  137. Singer, J. L. (1975). Navigating the stream of consciousness: Research in daydreaming and related inner experiences.American Psychologist,30, 727–738.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  138. Singer, J. L. (1978). Experimental studies of daydreaming and the stream of thought. In K. S. Pope & J. L. Singer (Eds.),The stream of consciousness: Scientific investigations into the flow of human experience (pp. 209–227). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  139. Squire, L. R. (1992). Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys and humans.Psychological Review,99, 195–231.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  140. Sternberg, R. J., &Lubart, T. I. (1995). An investment perspective on creative insight. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson (Eds.),The nature of insight (pp. 386–426). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  141. Sternberg, R. J., &Lubart, T. I. (1999). The concept of creativity: Prospects and paradigms. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.),Handbook of creativity (pp. 3–15). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  142. Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., &Knight, R. T. (1998). Frontal lobe contributions to the theory of mind.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,10, 640–656.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  143. Taylor, J. G. (2001). The central role of the parietal lobes in consciousness.Consciousness & Cognition,10, 379–417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  144. Taylor, S. F. (1996). Cerebral blood flow and functional lesions in schizophrenia.Schizophrenia Research,19, 129–140.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  145. Torrance, E. P. (1974).Torrance test of creative thinking. Lexington, MA: Personal Press.

    Google Scholar 

  146. Torrance, E. P. (1988). Intense emotional experiences-impetus for creation.Creative Child & Adult Quarterly,11, 130–137.

    Google Scholar 

  147. Torrance, E. P., &Hall, L. K. (1980). Assessing the further reaches of creative potential.Journal of Creative Behavior,14, 1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  148. Vogeley, K., Bussfeld, P., Newen, A., Herrman, S., Happe, F., Falkai, P., Maier, W., Shah, N. J., Fink, G. R., &Zilles, K. (2001). Mind reading: Neural mechanisms of theory of mind and selfperspective.NeuroImage,14, 170–181.

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  149. Vogeley, K., Kurthen, M., Falkai, P., &Maier, W. (1999). Essential functions of the human self model are implemented in the prefrontal cortex.Consciousness & Cognition,8, 343–363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  150. Waddell, C. (1998). Creativity and mental illness: Is there a link?Canadian Journal of Psychiatry,43, 166–172.

    Google Scholar 

  151. Ward, T. B., Smith, S. M., &Finke, R. A. (1999). Creative cognition. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.),Handbook of creativity (pp. 189–212). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  152. Weisberg, R. W. (1993).Creativity: Beyond the myth of genius. New York: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  153. Weisberg, R. W. (1999). Creativity and knowledge: A challenge to theories. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.),Handbook of creativity (pp. 226–250). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  154. Weisberg, R. W., &Alba, J. W. (1981). An examination of the alleged role of “fixation” in the solution of several “insight” problems.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,110, 169–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  155. Wertheimer, M. (1982).Productive thinking. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Arne Dietrich.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Dietrich, A. The cognitive neuroscience of creativity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 11, 1011–1026 (2004). https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196731

Download citation

Keywords

  • Prefrontal Cortex
  • Frontal Lobe
  • Creative Thinking
  • Cognitive Flexibility
  • Creativity Research