Skip to main content

Advertisement

SpringerLink
  • Log in
  1. Home
  2. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  3. Article
Gustave Caillebotte, French Impressionism, and mere exposure
Download PDF
Your article has downloaded

Similar articles being viewed by others

Slider with three articles shown per slide. Use the Previous and Next buttons to navigate the slides or the slide controller buttons at the end to navigate through each slide.

Challenge of the Ineffable: Concerning Mechner’s “a Behavioral and Biological Analysis of Aesthetics”

22 August 2018

Philip N. Hineline

A Functional Analysis of “Aesthetic”: A Commentary on Mechner

13 August 2018

Henry D. Schlinger Jr

A standardised set of images for judgements of proportion

15 September 2022

Sara Varatojo, Leonor Lavradio, … Teresa Garcia-Marques

Perception in Practice

08 April 2022

Dominic McIver Lopes & Madeleine Ransom

The center cannot hold: Variations of frame width help to explain the “inward bias” in aesthetic preferences

02 April 2021

Isabella R. Forman, Yi-Chia Chen, … George A. Alvarez

Infants look longer at colours that adults like when colours are highly saturated

17 December 2019

A. E. Skelton & A. Franklin

Behavioral Functions of Aesthetics: Science and Art, Reason, and Emotion

29 August 2018

Travis Thompson

Aesthetic perception and the puzzle of training

11 April 2022

Madeleine Ransom

Mechner’s Reply to the Commentaries on His Article, “A Behavioral and Biological Analysis of Aesthetics”

20 September 2018

Francis Mechner

Download PDF
  • Theoretical and Review Articles
  • Published: June 2003

Gustave Caillebotte, French Impressionism, and mere exposure

  • James E. Cutting1 

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review volume 10, pages 319–343 (2003)Cite this article

  • 4672 Accesses

  • 72 Citations

  • 1 Altmetric

  • Metrics details

Abstract

Gustave Caillebotte was a painter, a collector of some of his colleagues’ most renowned works, and a major force in the creation of the late 19th century French Impressionist canon. Six studies are presented as a naturalistic investigation of the effects of mere exposure to images in his collection and to those matched to them. The probabilities of cultural exposure to the 132 stimulus images were indexed by the frequencies of their separate appearances in Cornell University library books—a total of 4,232 times in 980 different books. Across the studies, adult preferences were correlated with differences in image frequencies, but not with recognition, complexity, or prototypicality judgments; children’s preferences were not correlated with frequency. Prior cultural exposure also interacted with experimental exposure in predictable ways. The results suggest that mere exposure helps to maintain an artistic canon.

Download to read the full article text

Working on a manuscript?

Avoid the common mistakes

References

  • Bal, M., &Bryson, N. (1991). Semiotics and art history.Art Bulletin,73, 174–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bazin, G. (1958).Impressionist paintings in the Louvre (S. Cunliffe-Owen, Trans.). London: Thames & Hudson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bérhaut, M. (1994).Gustave Caillebotte: Catalogue raisonné des peintures et pastels. Paris: La Bibliothèque des Arts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D. E. (1970). Novelty, complexity, and hedonic value.Perception & Psychophysics,8, 279–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernac, J. (1966). The Caillebotte bequest to the Luxembourg. Reprinted and translated in D. Sutton,Gustave Caillebotte, 1848–1894: A loan exhibition in aid of the Hertford British Hospital in Paris, 15th June–16th July, 1966. London: Wildenstein. (Original published in 1895)

    Google Scholar 

  • Bonnano, G. A., &Stilling, N. A. (1986). Preference, familiarity, and recognition after repeated brief exposure to random geometric shapes.American Journal of Psychology,99, 403–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, R. (1989). Exposure and affect: Overview and meta-analysis of research, 1968–1987.Psychological Bulletin,106, 265–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brickman, P., Redfield, J., Harrison, A. A., &Crandall, R. (1972). Drive and predisposition as factors in the attitudinal effects of mere exposure.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,8, 31–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chastel, A. (1971).Paris. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheetham, M. A. (2002).Kant, art, and art history. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, K. (1974).Another part of the wood. London: John Murray.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cutting, J., Wang, R. F., Flückiger, M., &Baumberger, B. (1999). Human heading judgments and object-based motion information.Vision Research,39, 1079–1105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Daulte, F. (1959).Alfred Sisley: Catalogue raisonné de l’oeuvre peint. Paris: Durand-Ruel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daulte, F. (1971).Auguste Renoir: Catalogue raisonné de l’oeuvre peint: Vol 1. Figures 1860–1890. Lausanne: Durand-Ruel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Distel, A. (1990).Impressionism: The first collectors. (B. Perroud-Benson, Trans.). New York: Harry N. Abrams.

    Google Scholar 

  • Distel, A. (Director) (1994).Gustave Caillebotte: 1848–1894 [Art exhibition]. Paris: Réunion des Musées Nationaux.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliott, R., &Dolan, R. J. (1998). Neural response during preference and memory judgments for subliminally presented stimuli: A functional neuroimaging study.Journal of Neuroscience,18, 4697–4704.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, R. (1999). Can we still use the canon?Art Journal,58, 4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fezzi, E. (1972).L’opera completa di Renoir [The complete works of Renoir]. Milan: Rizzoli Editore.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fox, B. (1983, October 20). Video thrills the Hollywood star.New Scientist,100, 203–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1947).Motion picture testing and research (Army Air Force Aviation Psychology Research Rep. 7). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, N. (1972).Problems and projects. Indianapolis, IN: Bobbs-Merrill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guillory, J. (1990). Canon. In F. Lentrichhia & T. McLaughlin (Eds.),Critical terms for literary study (pp. 233–262). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guillory, J. (1993).Cultural capital: The problem with literary canon formation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hallberg, R. von (Ed.) (1984).Canons. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, A. A. (1977). Mere exposure. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 39–83). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harrison, A. A., &Zajonc, R. B. (1970). Effects of frequency and duration of exposure on response competition and affect ratings.Journal of Psychology,75, 163–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayward Gallery (1985).Renoir. Catalog of the exhibit at the Hayward Gallery, London, 30 June–21 August 1985, Galérie Nationale de Grand Palais, Paris, 14 May–2 September 1985, Museum of Fine Arts, Boston, 9 October–5 January, 1986.

  • Jamot, P., &Wildenstein, G. (1932).Edouard Manet (Vols. 1 and 2). Paris: Les Beaux-arts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joseph, J. E., &Proffitt, D. R. (1996). Semantic versus perceptual influences of color on object recognition.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,22, 407–429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kant, I. (1952).Critique of aesthetic judgment. (J. Meredith, Trans.) Oxford: Oxford University Press, Clarendon Press. (Original work published 1794)

    Google Scholar 

  • Klinger, M. R., &Greenwald, A. (1994). Preferences need no inferences? The cognitive basis of unconscious mere exposure effects. In P. M. Niedenthal & S. Kitayama (Eds.),The heart’s eye: Emotional influences in perception and attention (pp. 67–85). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kozlowski, L. T., &Cutting, J. E. (1977). Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light display.Perception & Psychophysics,21, 575–580.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kunst-Wilson, W. R., &Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Affective discrimination of stimuli that cannot be recognized.Science,207, 557–558.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lemoisne, P. A. (1946).Degas et son oeuvre (Vols. 1–4). Paris: Arts et Métiers Graphiques.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, M. G., &Hakmiller, K. L. (1975). Effects of frequency, duration of study trials and total duration of exposure on affective judgments.Psychological Reports,37, 195–200.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marrinan, M. (2002). Caillebotte as a professional painter: From Studio to the public eye. In N. Broude (Ed.),Gustave Caillebotte and the fashioning of identity in Impressionist Paris (pp. 21–65). New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mead, K. H. (Director) (1974).The Impressionists and the salon (1874–1886) [Art exhibition]. Riverside, CA: Regents of the University of California.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monahan, J. L., Murphy, S. T., &Zajonc, R. B. (2000). Subliminal mere exposure: Specific, general, and diffuse effects.Psychological Science,11, 462–466.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moreland, R. L., &Zajonc, R. B. (1977). Is stimulus recognition a necessary condition for the occurrence of exposure effects?Journal of Social & Personality Psychology,35, 191–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moxey, K. (1994).The practice of theory: Poststructuralism, cultural politics, and art history. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Musée d’Orsay (1990).Catalogue sommaire illustré des peintures (Vols. 1 and 2). Paris: Réunion des Musées Nationaux.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nord, P. (2000).Impressionists and politics. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oliva, A., &Schyns, P. G. (2000). Diagnostic colors mediate scene recognition.Cognitive Psychology,41, 176–210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pirsig, R. (1974).Zen and the art of motorcycle maintenance. New York: Morrow.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pissarro, L.-R., &Venturi, L. (1939).Camille Pissarro - son art -son oeuvre (Vols. 1 and 2). Paris: Paul Rosenberg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rewald, J. (1946).The history of Impressionism. New York: Museum of Modern Art.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rewald, J. (1996).The paintings of Paul Cézanne: A catalogue raisonné (Vols. 1 and 2). New York: Harry N. Abrams.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., III (1990). Implicit memory: Retention without remembering.American Psychologist,45, 231–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E. H. (1973). Natural categories.Cognitive Psychology,4, 328–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberg, J. (1967).On quality in art: Criteria of excellence, past and present. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saltzman, C. (1998).Portrait of Dr. Gachet. New York: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sassoon, D. (2001).Becoming Mona Lisa. New York: Harcourt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,13, 501–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seamon, J. G., Brody, N., &Kauff, D. M. (1983). Affective discrimination of stimuli that are not recognized: II. Effect of delay between study and test.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society,21, 187–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seamon, J. G., &Delgado, M. R. (1999). Recognition memory and affective preference for depth-rotated solid objects: Part-based structural descriptions may underlie the mere exposure effect.Visual Cognition,6, 145–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seamon, J. G., Williams, P. C., Crowley, M. J., Kim, I. J., Langer, S.A., Orne, P. J., &Wishengrad, D. L. (1995). The mere exposure effect is based on implicit memory: Effects of stimulus type, encoding conditions, and number of exposures on recognition and affects judgments.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,21, 711–721.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shikes, R. E., &Harper, P. (1980).Pissarro: His life and work. New York: Horizon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. R. (1998). Mental representation and memory. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.),The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., pp. 391–445). Boston: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Squire, L. R. (1992). Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis of findings with rats, monkeys, and humans.Psychological Review,99, 195–231.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vanbeseleare, N. (1983). Mere exposure: A search for an explanation. In W. Doise & S. Moscovici (Eds.),Current issues in European social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 239–278). London: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varnedoe, K. (1987).Gustave Caillebotte. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Venturi, L. (1936).Cézanne, son art, son oeuvre (Vols. 1 and 2). Paris: Paul Rosenberg.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wadley, N. (Ed.) (1987).Renoir: A retrospective. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, B. E. (1984).Renoir: His life, art, and letters. New York: Harry N. Abrams.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wildenstein, D. (1974–1985).Claude Monet: Biographie et catalogue raisonné (Vols. 1–5). Paris: La Bibliothèque des Arts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winkielman, P., Zajonc, R. B., &Schwarz, N. (1997). Subliminal affective priming resists attributional interventions.Cognition & Emotion,11, 433–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodford, S. (1983).Looking at pictures. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wurm, L. H., Legge, G. E., Isenberg, L. M., &Luebker, A. (1993). Color improves object recognition in normal and low vision.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,19, 899–911.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure.Journal of Personality & Social Psychology,9, 1–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1970, February). Brainwash: Familiarity breeds comfort.Psychology Today,4, 33–35, 60–62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences.American Psychologist,35, 151–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (2001). Mere exposure: A gateway to the subliminal.Current Directions in Psychological Science,10, 224–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B., Crandall, R., Kail, R. V., &Swap, W. (1974). Effects of extreme exposure frequencies on different affective ratings of stimuli.Perceptual & Motor Skills,38, 667–678.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B., Shaver, P., Tavris, C., &van Kreveld, D. (1972). Exposure, satiation, and stimulus discriminability.Journal of Personality & Social Psychology,21, 270–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

  1. Department of Psychology, Cornell University, Uris Hall, 14853-7601, Ithaca, NY

    James E. Cutting

Authors
  1. James E. Cutting
    View author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

Additional information

This research began as a set of interests accrued in 1993, when I was in Paris, supported by a John Simon Guggenheim Memorial Fellowship, and later, in the summer of 1994, during the Caillebotte centennial. I thank Claudia Lazzaro for being the final, if unwitting, impetus for this study in the summer of 2000—again, in Paris—and for many wonderful hours of discussion about art and canons; Justine Zee Kwok for beginning aspects of this project as a feasibility study, for many hours of library work, and for help with initial assembly of the databases; Nicholas Epley for initial guidance through the mere exposure literature; John Bargh and Arthur Reber for encouragement and comments; Andrea Coby Riddle, the teachers, and students at the Elizabeth Ann Clune Montessori School of Ithaca for their help with Study 3; David Dunning and Dennis Regan for suggestions leading to Studies 4 and 5; and many colleagues for their friendly cajoling, forcing me to design and carry out Study 6. A version of Study 1 was presented at the 42nd Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, November 2001, in Orlando, FL.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Cutting, J.E. Gustave Caillebotte, French Impressionism, and mere exposure. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 10, 319–343 (2003). https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196493

Download citation

  • Received: 10 December 2001

  • Accepted: 17 May 2002

  • Issue Date: June 2003

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196493

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

Keywords

  • Image Pair
  • Comparison Image
  • General Text
  • Mere Exposure
  • Frequent Image
Download PDF

Working on a manuscript?

Avoid the common mistakes

Advertisement

Over 10 million scientific documents at your fingertips

Switch Edition
  • Academic Edition
  • Corporate Edition
  • Home
  • Impressum
  • Legal information
  • Privacy statement
  • California Privacy Statement
  • How we use cookies
  • Manage cookies/Do not sell my data
  • Accessibility
  • FAQ
  • Contact us
  • Affiliate program

Not logged in - 34.232.63.94

Not affiliated

Springer Nature

© 2023 Springer Nature Switzerland AG. Part of Springer Nature.