Skip to main content

Advertisement

SpringerLink
  • Log in
  1. Home
  2. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
  3. Article
Perceptual automaticity in expert chess players: Parallel encoding of chess relations
Download PDF
Download PDF
  • Brief Reports
  • Published: September 2001

Perceptual automaticity in expert chess players: Parallel encoding of chess relations

  • Eyal M. Reingold1,
  • Neil Charness2,
  • Richard S. Schultetus2 &
  • …
  • Dave M. Stampe1 

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review volume 8, pages 504–510 (2001)Cite this article

  • 962 Accesses

  • 64 Citations

  • Metrics details

Abstract

A check detection task in a 5 × 5 section of the chessboard, containing a King and one or two potential checking pieces was employed. The checking status (i.e., the presence or absence of a check) and the number of attackers (one or two) were manipulated. It was found that the reaction time cost for adding a distractor was differentially greater inno trials thanyes trials for novice, but not for expert, chess players. In addition, we contrasted standard check detection trials with trials in which one of two attackers was cued (colored red) and the task was to determine the checking status of the cued attacker while ignoring the other attacker. We documented a Stroop-like interference effect on trials in which a cued nonchecking attacker appeared together with an attacker that was checking (i.e., incongruent). These findings suggest automatic and parallel encoding procedures for chess relations in experts.

Download to read the full article text

Working on a manuscript?

Avoid the most common mistakes and prepare your manuscript for journal editors.

Learn more

References

  • Bachmann, T., &Oit, M. (1992). Stroop-like interference in chess players’ imagery: An unexplored possibility to be revealed by the adapted moving-spot task.Psychological Research,54, 27–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, J. (1978). The word-superiority effect: Perceptual learning from reading. In W. K. Estes (Ed.),Handbook of learning and cognitive processes (Vol. 6, pp. 131–166). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Besner, D., &Stolz, J. A. (1999a). Unconsciously controlled processing: The Stroop effect reconsidered.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,6, 449–455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Besner, D., &Stolz, J. A. (1999b). What kind of attention modulates the Stroop effect?Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,6, 99–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Besner, D., Stolz, J. A., &Boutilier, C. (1997). The Stroop effect and the myth of automaticity.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,4, 221–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biederman, I. (1972). Perceiving real world scenes.Science,177, 77–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Biederman, I. (1981). On the semantics of a glance at a scene. In M. Kubovy & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.),Perceptual organization (pp. 213–253). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biederman, I., Glass, A. L., &Stacey, E. W., Jr. (1973). Scanning for objects in real world scenes.Journal of Experimental Psychology,97, 22–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chase, W. G., &Simon, H. A. (1973a). The mind’s eye in chess. In W.G. Chase (Ed.),Visual information processing (pp. 215–281). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chase, W. G., &Simon, H. A. (1973b). Perception in chess.Cognitive Psychology,4, 55–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Church, R. M., &Church, K. W. (1983). Plans, goals, and search strategies for the selection of a move in chess. In P. W. Frey (Ed.),Chess skill in man and machine (2nd ed., pp. 131–156). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Groot, A. D. (1978).Thought and choice in chess. (2nd ed.). The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Groot, A. D., &Gobet, F. (1996).Perception and memory in chess. Assen, The Netherlands: Van Gorcum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, S. H. (1973). Structure and experience in the matching and reproduction of chess patterns.Dissertation Abstracts International, 34, 5B (University Microfilms No. AAG73-26954).

  • Frensch, P. A., &Sternberg, R. J. (1989). Expertise and intelligent thinking: When is it worse to know better? In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.),Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 5, pp. 157–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gobet, F., &Simon, H. A. (1996a). Recall of random and distorted chess positions: Implications for the theory of expertise.Memory & Cognition,24, 493–503.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gobet, F., &Simon, H. A. (1996b). Templates in chess memory: A mechanism for recalling several boards.Cognitive Psychology,31, 1–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gobet, F., &Simon, H. A. (2000). Five seconds or sixty? Presentation time in expert memory.Cognitive Science,24, 651–682.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gyoba, J., Arimura, M., &Maruyama, K. (1980). Visual identification of line segments embedded in human face patterns.Tohoku Psychologica Folia,39, 113–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Homa, D., Haver, B., &Schwartz, T. (1976). Perceptibility of schematic face stimuli: Evidence for a perceptual Gestalt.Memory & Cognition,4, 176–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory.Journal of Memory & Language,30, 513–541.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L., Ste-Marie, D., &Toth, J. T. (1993). Redefining automaticity: Unconscious influences, awareness and control. In A. D. Baddeley & L. Weiskrantz (Eds.),Attention, selection, awareness and control: A tribute to Donald Broadbent (pp. 261–282). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, J. C. (1981). Understanding word perception: Clues from studying the word-superiority effect. In O. J. L. Tzeng & H. Singer (Eds.),Perception of print: Reading research in experimental psychology (pp. 65–84). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jongman, R. W. (1968).Het oog van de meester [The eye of the master]. Assen, The Netherlands: Van Gorcum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, L. E. (1975). Familiarity effects in visual information processing.Psychological Bulletin,82, 949–974.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C. M. (1992). The Stroop task: The “gold standard” of attentional measures.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,121, 12–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L. (1979). On the time relations of mental processes: A framework for analyzing processes in cascade.Psychological Review,86, 287–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McConkie, G. W., &Rayner, K. (1975). The span of the effective stimulus during a fixation in reading.Perception & Psychophysics,17, 578–586.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milojkovic, J. D. (1982). Chess imagery in novice and master.Journal of Mental Imagery,6, 125–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, S. E. (1975). The effects of contextual scenes on the identification of objects.Memory & Cognition,3, 519–526.

    Google Scholar 

  • Purcell, D. G., &Stewart, A. L. (1986). The face-detection effect.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society,24, 118–120.

    Google Scholar 

  • Purcell, D. G., &Stewart, A. L. (1988). The face-detection effect: Configuration enhances detection.Perception & Psychophysics,43, 355–366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research.Psychological Bulletin,124, 372–422.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reingold, E. M. (1995). Facilitation and interference in indirect/implicit memory tests and in the process dissociation paradigm: The letter insertion and the letter deletion tasks.Consciousness & Cognition,4, 459–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reingold, E. M., Charness, N., Pomplun, M., &Stampe, D. M. (2001). Visual span in expert chess players: Evidence from eye movements.Psychological Science,12, 49–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reingold, E. M., &Jolicoeur, P. (1993). Perceptual versus postperceptual mediation of visual context effects: Evidence from the lettersuperiority effect.Perception & Psychophysics,53, 166–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reingold, E. M., &Toth, J. P. (1996). Process dissociations versus task dissociations: A controversy in progress. In G. Underwood (Ed.),Implicit cognition (pp. 159–202). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rensink, R. A., O’Regan, J. K., &Clark, J. J. (1997). To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes.Psychological Science,8, 368–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saariluoma, P. (1984).Coding problem spaces in chess. (Commentationes Scientiarum Socialium, Vol. 23). Turku: Societas Scientiarum Fennica.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saariluoma, P. (1985). Chess players’ intake of task-relevant cues.Memory & Cognition,13, 385–391.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saariluoma, P. (1990). Apperception and restructuring in chess players’ problem solving. In K. J. Gilhooly, M. T. G. Keane, R. H. Logie, & G. Erdos (Eds.),Lines of thought: Reflections on the psychology of thinking (pp. 41–57). London: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saariluoma, P. (1994). Location coding in chess.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,47A, 607–630.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schendel, J. D., &Shaw, P. (1976). A test of the generality of the word-context effect.Perception & Psychophysics,19, 383–393.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A., &Barenfeld, M. (1969). Information-processing analysis of perceptual processes in problem solving.Psychological Review,76, 473–483.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A., &Gilmartin, K. (1973). A simulation of memory for chess positions.Cognitive Psychology,5, 29–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simons, D. J., &Levin, D. T. (1997). Change blindness.Trends in Cognitive Sciences,1, 261–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stolz, J. A., &Besner, D. (1996). Role of set in visual word recognition: Activation and activation blocking as nonautomatic processes.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,22,1166–11777.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stolz, J. A., &Besner, D. (1999). On the myth of automatic semantic activation in reading.Current Directions in Psychological Science,8, 61–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions.Journal of Experimental Psychology,18, 643–661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tikhomirov, O. K., &Poznyanskaya, E. (1966). An investigation of visual search as a means of analyzing heuristics.Soviet Psychology,5, 2–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toth, J. P., Reingold, E. M., &Jacoby, L. L. (1994). Toward a redefinition of implicit memory: Process dissociations following elaborative processing and self-generation.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,20, 290–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A., &Gelade, G. (1980). A feature integration theory of attention.Cognitive Psychology,12, 97–136.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Santen, J. P. H., &Jonides, J. (1978). A replication of the facesuperiority effect.Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society,12, 378–380.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winikoff, A. W. (1967).Eye movements as an aid to protocol analysis of problem solving behavior. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Carnegie-Mellon University, Pittsburgh.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M. (1998). Visual search. In H. Pashler (Ed.),Attention (pp. 13–73). Hove, U.K.: Psychology Press/Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

  1. Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, 100 St. George Street, M5S 3G3, Toronto, ON, Canada

    Eyal M. Reingold & Dave M. Stampe

  2. Florida State University, Tallahassee, Florida

    Neil Charness & Richard S. Schultetus

Authors
  1. Eyal M. Reingold
    View author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

  2. Neil Charness
    View author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

  3. Richard S. Schultetus
    View author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

  4. Dave M. Stampe
    View author publications

    You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Eyal M. Reingold.

Additional information

Preparation of this paper was supported by a grant to E.M.R. from the Natural Science and Engineering Research Council of Canada and by NIA Grant 5R01 AG13969 to N.C.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Reingold, E.M., Charness, N., Schultetus, R.S. et al. Perceptual automaticity in expert chess players: Parallel encoding of chess relations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 8, 504–510 (2001). https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196185

Download citation

  • Received: 13 April 2000

  • Accepted: 04 December 2000

  • Issue Date: September 2001

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196185

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

Keywords

  • Stroop Effect
  • Congruent Condition
  • Check Status
  • Skilled Player
  • Reaction Time Cost
Download PDF

Working on a manuscript?

Avoid the most common mistakes and prepare your manuscript for journal editors.

Learn more

Advertisement

Over 10 million scientific documents at your fingertips

Switch Edition
  • Academic Edition
  • Corporate Edition
  • Home
  • Impressum
  • Legal information
  • Privacy statement
  • California Privacy Statement
  • How we use cookies
  • Manage cookies/Do not sell my data
  • Accessibility
  • FAQ
  • Contact us
  • Affiliate program

Not logged in - 3.238.134.157

Not affiliated

Springer Nature

© 2023 Springer Nature Switzerland AG. Part of Springer Nature.