Abstract
This article reports five experiments demonstrating theoretically coherent effects of emotion on memory and attention. Experiments 1–3 demonstrated three taboo Stroop effects that occur when people name the color of taboo words. One effect is longer color-naming times for taboo than for neutral words, an effect that diminishes with word repetition. The second effect is superior recall of taboo words in surprise memory tests following color naming. The third effect is better recognition memory for colors consistently associated with taboo words rather than with neutral words. None of these effects was due to retrieval factors, attentional disengagement processes, response inhibition, or strategic attention shifts. Experiments 4 and 5 demonstrated that taboo words impair immediate recall of the preceding and succeeding words in rapidly presented lists but do not impair lexical decision times. We argue that taboo words trigger specific emotional reactions that facilitate the binding of taboo word meaning to salient contextual aspects, such as occurrence in a task and font color in taboo Stroop tasks.
Article PDF
Similar content being viewed by others
Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.
References
Bargh, J. A. (1992). The ecology of automaticity: Toward establishing the conditions needed to produce automatic processing effects.American Journal of Psychology,105, 181–200.
Bellezza, F. S., Greenwald, A. G., &Banaji, M. R. (1986). Words high and low in pleasantness as rated by male and female college students.Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers,18, 299–303.
Berlyne, D. E. (1960).Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Bower, G. H. (1992). How might emotions affect learning? In S. Christianson (Ed.),Handbook of emotion and memory: Research and theory (pp. 3–32). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cahill, L., &McGaugh, J. L. (1995). A novel demonstration of enhanced memory associated with emotional arousal.Consciousness & Cognition,4, 410–421.
Canli, T., Zhao, Z., Brewer, J., Gabrieli, J. D., &Cahill, L. (2000). Event-related activation in the human amygdala associates with later memory for individual emotional response [Abstract].Journal of Neuroscience,20, RC99.
Conway, M. A., Anderson, S. J., Larsen, S. F., Donnelly, C. M., McDaniel, M. A., McClelland, A. G. R., Rowles, R. E., &Logie, R. H. (1994). The formation of flashbulb memories.Memory & Cognition,22, 326–343.
Dalgleish, T., &Watts, F. N. (1990). Biases of attention and memory in disorders of anxiety and depression.Clinical Psychology Review,10, 589–604.
Halgren, E. (1992). Emotional neurophysiology of the amygdala within the context of human cognition. In J. P. Aggleton (Ed.),The amygdala (pp. 191–228). New York: Wiley-Liss.
Hamann, S. (2001). Cognitive and neural mechanisms of emotional memory.Trends in Cognitive Sciences,5, 394–400.
Hamann, S. B., Ely, T. D., Grafton, S. T., &Kilts, C. D. (1999). Amygdala activity related to enhanced memory for pleasant and aversive stimuli.Nature Neuroscience,2, 289–293.
Hebb, D. O. (1946). On the nature of fear.Psychological Review,53, 259–276.
Jay, T. B. (1992).Cursing in America. Philadelphia: Benjamins.
Jay, T. B. (2000).Why we curse: A neuro-psycho-social theory of speech. Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
LaBar, K. S., &Phelps, E. A. (1998). Arousal-mediated memory consolidation: Role of the medial temporal lobe in humans.Psychological Science,9, 490–493.
Larsen, S. F. (1992). Potential flashbulbs: Memories of ordinary news as the baseline. In E. Winograd & U. Neisser (Eds.),Affect and accuracy in recall (pp. 32–64). New York: Cambridge University Press.
MacKay, D. G., Burke, D. M., &Stewart, R. (1998). H.M.’s language production deficits: Implications for relations between memory, semantic binding, and the hippocampal system.Journal of Memory & Language,38, 28–69.
MacKay, D. G., &James, L. E. (2001). The binding problem for syntax, semantics, and prosody: H.M.’s selective sentence-reading deficits under the theoretical-syndrome approach.Language & Cognitive Processes,16, 419–460.
MacKay, D. G., &James, L. E. (2003). Aging, retrograde amnesia, and the binding problem for phonology and orthography: A longitudinal study of “hippocampal amnesic” H.M.Aging, Neuropsychology & Cognition,9, 298–333.
MacKay, D. G., Miller, M. D., &Schuster, S. P. (1994). Repetition blindness and aging: Evidence for a binding deficit involving a single theoretically-specified connection.Psychology & Aging,9, 251–258.
MacKay, D. G., Stewart, R., &Burke, D. M. (1998). H.M. revisited: Relations between language comprehension, memory, and the hippocampal system.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,10, 377–394.
MacLeod, C. M., &Hodder, S. L. (1998). On the difficulty in obtaining priming effects in the Stroop task.Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society,3, 37.
Mandler, G. (1975).Mind and emotion. New York: Wiley.
Mathews, A., &MacLeod, C. (1985). Discrimination of threat cues without awareness in anxiety states.Journal of Abnormal Psychology,95, 131–138.
Matthews, G., &Harley, T. A. (1996). Connectionist models of emotional distress and attentional bias.Cognition & Emotion,10, 561–600.
McClelland, D. C. (1951).Personality. New York: Sloan.
McKenna, F. P., &Sharma, D. (1995). Intrusive cognitions: An investigation of the emotional Stroop task.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,21, 1595–1607.
Murdock, B. B. (1974).Human memory: Theory and data. Potomac, MD: Erlbaum.
Neisser, U., Winograd, E., Bergman, E. T., Schreiber, C. A., Palmer, S. E., &Weldon, M. S. (1996). Remembering the earthquake: Direct experience vs. hearing the news.Memory,4, 337–357.
Nielson, S. L., &Sarason, I. G. (1981). Emotion, personality, and selective attention.Journal of Personality & Social Psychology,41, 945–960.
Phelps, E. A., &LaBar, K. S. (1997). The role of organization in recall for affective words.Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society,2, 4–5.
Pratto, F., &John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attentiongrabbing power of negative social information.Journal of Personality & Social Psychology,61, 380–391.
Reiman, B. C., &McNally, R. J. (1995). Cognitive processing of personally relevant information.Cognition & Emotion,9, 325–340.
Richards, A., French, C. C., Johnson, W., Naparstek, J., &Williams, J. (1992). Effects of mood manipulation and anxiety on performance of an emotional Stroop task.British Journal of Psychology,83, 479–491.
Richards, A., &Millwood, B. (1989). Colour-identification of differentially valenced words in anxiety.Cognition & Emotion,3, 171–176.
Rundus, D. (1971). Analysis of rehearsal processes in free recall.Journal of Experimental Psychology,89, 63–77.
Schacter, D. L. (1999). The seven sins of memory: Insights from psychology and cognitive neuroscience.American Psychologist,54, 182–203.
Shapiro, K. L., Caldwell, J., &Sorenson, R. E. (1997). Personal names and the attentional blink: A visual “cocktail party” effect.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,23, 504–514.
Sharma, D., &McKenna, F. P. (2001). The role of time pressure on the emotional Stroop task.British Journal of Psychology,92, 471–481.
Siegrist, M. (1995). Effects of taboo words on color-naming performance on a Stroop test.Perceptual & Motor Skills,81, 1119–1122.
Talarico, J. M., &Rubin, D. C. (2003). Confidence, not consistency, characterizes flashbulb memories.Psychological Science,14, 455–461.
Tulving, E. (1969). Retrograde amnesia in free recall.Science,164, 88–90.
Tulving, E. (1985). How many memory systems are there?American Psychologist,40, 385–398.
van Selst, M., &Jolicoeur, P. (1994). A solution to the effect of sample size on outlier elimination.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,47A, 631–650.
Watts, F. N., McKenna, F. P., Sharrock, R., &Tresize, L. (1986). Processing of phobic stimuli.British Journal of Clinical Psychology,25, 253–261.
Wells, A., &Matthews, G. (1994).Attention and emotion: A clinical perspective. Hove, U.K.: Erlbaum.
Whalen, P. J., Bush, G., McNally, R. J., Wilhelm, S., McInerney, S. C., Jenike, M. A., &Rausch, S. L. (1998). The emotional counting Stroop paradigm: A functional magnetic resonance imaging probe of the anterior cingulate affective division.Biological Psychiatry,12, 1219–1228.
Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., &MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology.Psychological Bulletin,120, 2–24.
Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., MacLeod, C., &Watts, F. N. (1988).Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders. Chichester, U.K.: Wiley.
Wright, C. I., Fischer, H., Whalen, P. J., McInerney, S. C., Shin, L. M., &Rausch, S. L. (2001). Differential prefrontal cortex and amygdala habituation to repeatedly presented emotional stimuli.Neuro-Report,12, 379–383.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
This article is dedicated to Wayne Wickelgren (1938-).
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Mackay, D.G., Shafto, M., Taylor, J.K. et al. Relations between emotion, memory, and attention: Evidence from taboo Stroop, lexical decision, and immediate memory tasks. Memory & Cognition 32, 474–488 (2004). https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195840
Received:
Accepted:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195840