Advertisement

Memory & Cognition

, Volume 32, Issue 3, pp 474–488 | Cite as

Relations between emotion, memory, and attention: Evidence from taboo Stroop, lexical decision, and immediate memory tasks

  • Donald G. MackayEmail author
  • Meredith Shafto
  • Jennifer K. Taylor
  • Diane E. Marian
  • Lise Abrams
  • Jennifer R. Dyer
Article

Abstract

This article reports five experiments demonstrating theoretically coherent effects of emotion on memory and attention. Experiments 1–3 demonstrated three taboo Stroop effects that occur when people name the color of taboo words. One effect is longer color-naming times for taboo than for neutral words, an effect that diminishes with word repetition. The second effect is superior recall of taboo words in surprise memory tests following color naming. The third effect is better recognition memory for colors consistently associated with taboo words rather than with neutral words. None of these effects was due to retrieval factors, attentional disengagement processes, response inhibition, or strategic attention shifts. Experiments 4 and 5 demonstrated that taboo words impair immediate recall of the preceding and succeeding words in rapidly presented lists but do not impair lexical decision times. We argue that taboo words trigger specific emotional reactions that facilitate the binding of taboo word meaning to salient contextual aspects, such as occurrence in a task and font color in taboo Stroop tasks.

Keywords

Lexical Decision Lexical Decision Task Neutral Word Rapid Serial Visual Presentation Stroop Effect 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Bargh, J. A. (1992). The ecology of automaticity: Toward establishing the conditions needed to produce automatic processing effects.American Journal of Psychology,105, 181–200.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Bellezza, F. S., Greenwald, A. G., &Banaji, M. R. (1986). Words high and low in pleasantness as rated by male and female college students.Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers,18, 299–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Berlyne, D. E. (1960).Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. New York: McGraw-Hill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bower, G. H. (1992). How might emotions affect learning? In S. Christianson (Ed.),Handbook of emotion and memory: Research and theory (pp. 3–32). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  5. Cahill, L., &McGaugh, J. L. (1995). A novel demonstration of enhanced memory associated with emotional arousal.Consciousness & Cognition,4, 410–421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Canli, T., Zhao, Z., Brewer, J., Gabrieli, J. D., &Cahill, L. (2000). Event-related activation in the human amygdala associates with later memory for individual emotional response [Abstract].Journal of Neuroscience,20, RC99.Google Scholar
  7. Conway, M. A., Anderson, S. J., Larsen, S. F., Donnelly, C. M., McDaniel, M. A., McClelland, A. G. R., Rowles, R. E., &Logie, R. H. (1994). The formation of flashbulb memories.Memory & Cognition,22, 326–343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Dalgleish, T., &Watts, F. N. (1990). Biases of attention and memory in disorders of anxiety and depression.Clinical Psychology Review,10, 589–604.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Halgren, E. (1992). Emotional neurophysiology of the amygdala within the context of human cognition. In J. P. Aggleton (Ed.),The amygdala (pp. 191–228). New York: Wiley-Liss.Google Scholar
  10. Hamann, S. (2001). Cognitive and neural mechanisms of emotional memory.Trends in Cognitive Sciences,5, 394–400.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Hamann, S. B., Ely, T. D., Grafton, S. T., &Kilts, C. D. (1999). Amygdala activity related to enhanced memory for pleasant and aversive stimuli.Nature Neuroscience,2, 289–293.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Hebb, D. O. (1946). On the nature of fear.Psychological Review,53, 259–276.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Jay, T. B. (1992).Cursing in America. Philadelphia: Benjamins.Google Scholar
  14. Jay, T. B. (2000).Why we curse: A neuro-psycho-social theory of speech. Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
  15. LaBar, K. S., &Phelps, E. A. (1998). Arousal-mediated memory consolidation: Role of the medial temporal lobe in humans.Psychological Science,9, 490–493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Larsen, S. F. (1992). Potential flashbulbs: Memories of ordinary news as the baseline. In E. Winograd & U. Neisser (Eds.),Affect and accuracy in recall (pp. 32–64). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  17. MacKay, D. G., Burke, D. M., &Stewart, R. (1998). H.M.’s language production deficits: Implications for relations between memory, semantic binding, and the hippocampal system.Journal of Memory & Language,38, 28–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. MacKay, D. G., &James, L. E. (2001). The binding problem for syntax, semantics, and prosody: H.M.’s selective sentence-reading deficits under the theoretical-syndrome approach.Language & Cognitive Processes,16, 419–460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. MacKay, D. G., &James, L. E. (2003). Aging, retrograde amnesia, and the binding problem for phonology and orthography: A longitudinal study of “hippocampal amnesic” H.M.Aging, Neuropsychology & Cognition,9, 298–333.Google Scholar
  20. MacKay, D. G., Miller, M. D., &Schuster, S. P. (1994). Repetition blindness and aging: Evidence for a binding deficit involving a single theoretically-specified connection.Psychology & Aging,9, 251–258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. MacKay, D. G., Stewart, R., &Burke, D. M. (1998). H.M. revisited: Relations between language comprehension, memory, and the hippocampal system.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,10, 377–394.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. MacLeod, C. M., &Hodder, S. L. (1998). On the difficulty in obtaining priming effects in the Stroop task.Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society,3, 37.Google Scholar
  23. Mandler, G. (1975).Mind and emotion. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  24. Mathews, A., &MacLeod, C. (1985). Discrimination of threat cues without awareness in anxiety states.Journal of Abnormal Psychology,95, 131–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Matthews, G., &Harley, T. A. (1996). Connectionist models of emotional distress and attentional bias.Cognition & Emotion,10, 561–600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. McClelland, D. C. (1951).Personality. New York: Sloan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. McKenna, F. P., &Sharma, D. (1995). Intrusive cognitions: An investigation of the emotional Stroop task.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,21, 1595–1607.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Murdock, B. B. (1974).Human memory: Theory and data. Potomac, MD: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  29. Neisser, U., Winograd, E., Bergman, E. T., Schreiber, C. A., Palmer, S. E., &Weldon, M. S. (1996). Remembering the earthquake: Direct experience vs. hearing the news.Memory,4, 337–357.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Nielson, S. L., &Sarason, I. G. (1981). Emotion, personality, and selective attention.Journal of Personality & Social Psychology,41, 945–960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Phelps, E. A., &LaBar, K. S. (1997). The role of organization in recall for affective words.Abstracts of the Psychonomic Society,2, 4–5.Google Scholar
  32. Pratto, F., &John, O. P. (1991). Automatic vigilance: The attentiongrabbing power of negative social information.Journal of Personality & Social Psychology,61, 380–391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Reiman, B. C., &McNally, R. J. (1995). Cognitive processing of personally relevant information.Cognition & Emotion,9, 325–340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Richards, A., French, C. C., Johnson, W., Naparstek, J., &Williams, J. (1992). Effects of mood manipulation and anxiety on performance of an emotional Stroop task.British Journal of Psychology,83, 479–491.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Richards, A., &Millwood, B. (1989). Colour-identification of differentially valenced words in anxiety.Cognition & Emotion,3, 171–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Rundus, D. (1971). Analysis of rehearsal processes in free recall.Journal of Experimental Psychology,89, 63–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Schacter, D. L. (1999). The seven sins of memory: Insights from psychology and cognitive neuroscience.American Psychologist,54, 182–203.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Shapiro, K. L., Caldwell, J., &Sorenson, R. E. (1997). Personal names and the attentional blink: A visual “cocktail party” effect.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,23, 504–514.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Sharma, D., &McKenna, F. P. (2001). The role of time pressure on the emotional Stroop task.British Journal of Psychology,92, 471–481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Siegrist, M. (1995). Effects of taboo words on color-naming performance on a Stroop test.Perceptual & Motor Skills,81, 1119–1122.Google Scholar
  41. Talarico, J. M., &Rubin, D. C. (2003). Confidence, not consistency, characterizes flashbulb memories.Psychological Science,14, 455–461.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Tulving, E. (1969). Retrograde amnesia in free recall.Science,164, 88–90.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Tulving, E. (1985). How many memory systems are there?American Psychologist,40, 385–398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. van Selst, M., &Jolicoeur, P. (1994). A solution to the effect of sample size on outlier elimination.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,47A, 631–650.Google Scholar
  45. Watts, F. N., McKenna, F. P., Sharrock, R., &Tresize, L. (1986). Processing of phobic stimuli.British Journal of Clinical Psychology,25, 253–261.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Wells, A., &Matthews, G. (1994).Attention and emotion: A clinical perspective. Hove, U.K.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  47. Whalen, P. J., Bush, G., McNally, R. J., Wilhelm, S., McInerney, S. C., Jenike, M. A., &Rausch, S. L. (1998). The emotional counting Stroop paradigm: A functional magnetic resonance imaging probe of the anterior cingulate affective division.Biological Psychiatry,12, 1219–1228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., &MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology.Psychological Bulletin,120, 2–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., MacLeod, C., &Watts, F. N. (1988).Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders. Chichester, U.K.: Wiley.Google Scholar
  50. Wright, C. I., Fischer, H., Whalen, P. J., McInerney, S. C., Shin, L. M., &Rausch, S. L. (2001). Differential prefrontal cortex and amygdala habituation to repeatedly presented emotional stimuli.Neuro-Report,12, 379–383.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2004

Authors and Affiliations

  • Donald G. Mackay
    • 1
    Email author
  • Meredith Shafto
    • 1
  • Jennifer K. Taylor
    • 1
  • Diane E. Marian
    • 1
  • Lise Abrams
    • 1
  • Jennifer R. Dyer
    • 1
  1. 1.Psychology Dept.UCLALos Angeles

Personalised recommendations