Advertisement

Memory & Cognition

, Volume 31, Issue 8, pp 1229–1237 | Cite as

Age differences in enumerating things that move: Implications for the development of multiple-object tracking

  • Lana M. Trick
  • Diana Audet
  • Lynn Dales
Article

Abstract

The attentional theory of spatial enumeration (Trick & Pylyshyn, 1994) predicts that subitizing, the rapid process (40–120 msec/item) used to enumerate 1–4 items, employs the same mechanism that permits individuals to track 4–5 moving items simultaneously, whereas enumerating more items requires moving attentional focus from area to area in the display. To test this theory, enumeration of static and moving items was investigated in 8-, 10-, 12-, and 20-year-old participants using a number discrimination task. As was predicted, random independent item motion did not substantially impede enumeration of 1–4 items regardless of age. However, even movement within a 1.14° square area slowed enumeration of 6–9 items, although on average the interference decreased with age from 788 msec for the 8-year-olds to 136 msec for the 20-year-olds. The relevance of this finding for theories of enumeration, multiple-object tracking, visual working memory, and object-based attention is discussed.

Keywords

Attentional Focus Apparent Motion Work Memory Span Attentional Theory Counting Range 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Chi, M. T., &Klahr, D. (1975). Span and rate of apprehension in children and adults.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,19, 434–439.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Christ, S. E., McCrae, C. S., &Abrams, R. A. (2002). Inhibition of return in static and dynamic displays.Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,9, 80–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Folk, C. L., Egeth, H., &Kwak, H.-W. (1988). Subitizing: Direct apprehension or serial processing?Perception & Psychophysics,44, 313–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Gallistel, C. R., &Gelman, R. (1992). Preverbal and verbal counting and computation.Cognition,44, 43–74.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Jevons, W. (1871). The power of numerical discrimination.Nature,3, 281–282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Jiang, Y., Chun, M., &Marks, L. (2002a). Visual marking: Dissociating effects of new and old set size.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition,28, 293–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Jiang, Y., Chun, M., &Marks, L. (2002b). Visual marking: Selective attention to asynchronous temporal groups.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,28, 717–730.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Kaye, B., &Ruskin, E. (1990). The development of attentional control mechanisms. In J. T. Enns (Ed.),The development of attention: Research and theory (pp. 227–244). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Klahr, D., &Wallace, J. G. (1976).Cognitive development: An information processing view. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  10. Klein, R.M., &MacInnes, W. J. (1999). Inhibition of return is a foraging facilitator in visual search.Psychological Science,10, 346–352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Liss, P., &Reeves, A. (1983). Interruption of dot processing by a backward mask.Perception,12, 513–529.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Mandler, G., &Shebo, B. J. (1982). Subitizing: An analysis of its component processes.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,111, 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. McCrae, C. S., &Abrams, R. A. (2001). Age-related differences in object- and location-based inhibition of return of attention.Psychology & Aging,16, 437–449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Oyama, T., Kikuchi, T., &Ichihara, S. (1981). Span of attention, backward masking, and reaction time.Perception & Psychophysics,29, 106–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Pylyshyn, Z. W. (2001). Visual indexes, preconceptual objects, and situated vision.Cognition,80, 127–158.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Pylyshyn, Z. W., Elcock, E. W., Marmor, M., & Sander, P. (1978). Explorations in visual-motor spaces. InProceedings of the Second International Conference of the Canadian Society for the Computational Studies of Intelligence. University of Toronto.Google Scholar
  17. Pylyshyn, Z. W., &Storm, R. W. (1988). Tracking multiple independent targets: Evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism.Spatial Vision,3, 179–197.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Sagi, D., &Julesz, B. (1984). Detection versus discrimination of visual orientation.Perception,13, 619–628.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Sathian, K., Simon, T. J., Peterson, S., Patel, G. A., Hoffman, J.M., &Grafton, T. S. (1999). Neural evidence linking visual object enumeration and attention.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,11, 36–51.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Sears, C. R., &Pylyshyn, Z. W. (2000). Multiple-object tracking and attentional processing.Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology,54, 1–14.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Snyder, J. J., &Kingstone, A. (2000). Inhibition of return and visual search: How many separate loci are inhibited?Perception & Psychophysics,62, 452–458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Starkey, P., &Cooper, R. (1995). The development of subitizing in young children.British Journal of Developmental Psychology,13, 399–420.Google Scholar
  23. Tipper, S. P., Jordan, H., &Weaver, B. (1999). Scene-based and object-centered inhibition of return: Evidence for dual orienting mechanisms.Perception & Psychophysics,61, 50–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Tipper, S. P., Rafal, R., Reuter-Lorenz, P.A., Starrveldt, Y., Ro, T., Egly, R., Danziger, S., &Weaver, B. (1997). Object-based facilitation and inhibition from visual orienting in the human split-brain.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,23, 1522–1532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Tipper, S. P., Weaver, B., Jerreat, L., &Burak, A. (1994). Objectand environment-based inhibition of return in visual attention.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance,20, 478–499.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Trick, L.M. (2003, June).Age-related changes in the effect of response uncertainty. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the Canadian Society of Brain, Behaviour, and Cognitive Science, Hamilton, Ontario.Google Scholar
  27. Trick, L.M., Dales, L., & Audet, D. (2002, June). Enumerating items as they move: Item individuation and tracking. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the Canadian Society of Brain, Behaviour, and Cognitive Science, Vancouver.Google Scholar
  28. Trick, L.M., &Enns, J. T. (1998). Life-span changes in attention: The visual search task.Cognitive Development,13, 369–386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Trick, L.M., Enns, J. T., &Brodeur, D. A. (1996). Lifespan changes in visual enumeration: The number discrimination task.Developmental Psychology,32, 925–932.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Trick, L.M., &Pylyshyn, Z. P. (1994). Why are small and large numbers enumerated differently? A limited capacity preattentive stage in vision.Psychological Review,101, 80–102.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Tuholski, S. W., Engle, R.W., &Baylis, G.C. (2001). Individual differences in working memory capacity and enumeration.Memory & Cognition,29, 484–492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Ullman, S. (1984). Visual routines.Cognition,18(1–3), 97–159.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. van Oeffelen, M. P., &Vos, P. G. (1982). Configurational effects on the enumeration of dots: Counting by groups.Memory & Cognition,10, 396–404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Watson, D. G. (2001). Visual marking in moving displays: Featurebased inhibition is not necessary.Perception & Psychophysics,63, 74–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Watson, D. G., &Humphreys, G. W. (1997). Visual marking: Prioritizing selection for new objects by top-down attentional inhibition of old objects.Psychological Review,104, 90–122.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Watson, D. G., &Humphreys, G. W. (2000). Visual marking: Evidence for inhibition using a probe-dot detection paradigm.Perception & Psychophysics,62, 471–481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Watson, D. G., &Maylor, E. A. (2002). Aging and visual marking: Selective deficits for moving stimuli.Psychology & Aging,17, 321–339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2003

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of GuelphGuelphCanada

Personalised recommendations