Advertisement

Behavior Research Methods

, Volume 38, Issue 4, pp 698–703 | Cite as

A complete database of international chess players and chess performance ratings for varied longitudinal studies

  • Robert W. Howard
Article

Abstract

Chess is an oft-used study domain in psychology and artificial intelligence because it is well defined, its performance rating systems allow easy identification of experts and their development, and chess playing is a complex intellectual task. However, usable computerized chess data have been very limited. The present article has two aims. The first is to highlight the methodological value of chess data and how researchers can use them to address questions in quite different areas. The second is to present a computerized database of all international chess players and official performance ratings beginning from the inaugural 1970 international rating list. The database has millions of records and gives complete longitudinal official performance data for over 60,000 players from 1970 to the present. Like a time series of population censuses, these data can be used for many different research and teaching purposes. Three quite different studies, conducted by the author using the database, are described.

Keywords

Chess Player Performance Rating System Female Player Rating List Select Query 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Atherton, M., Zhuang, J., Bart, W. M., Hu, X., &He, S. (2003). A functional MRI study of high-level cognition: I. The game of chess.Cognitive Brain Research,16, 26–31.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Barrett, L. (2002). Do chess and go need “g”?Trends in Cognitive Sciences,6, 499.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Burgess, G. (1999).Chess highlights of the 20th century: The best chess 1900–1999 in historical context. London: Gambit.Google Scholar
  4. Burns, B. D. (2004). The effects of speed on skilled chess performance.Psychological Science,15, 442–447.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Chabris, C. F., &Hearst, E. S. (2003). Visualization, pattern recognition, and forward search: Effects of playing speed and sight of the position on grandmaster chess errors.Cognitive Science,27, 637–648.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Charness, N. (1992). The impact of chess research on cognitive science.Psychological Research,54, 4–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Charness, N., &Gerchak, Y. (1996). Participation rates and maximal performance: A log-linear explanation for group differences, such as Russian and male dominance in chess.Psychological Science,7, 46–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Charness, N., Krampe, R., &Mayr, U. (1996). The role of practice and coaching in entrepreneurial skill domains: An international comparison of life-span chess skill acquisition. In K. A. Ericsson (Ed.),The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts and sciences, sports, and games (pp. 51–80). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  9. Charness, N., Tuffiash, M., Krampe, R., Reingold, E., &Vasyukova, E. (2005). The role of deliberate practice in chess expertise.Applied Cognitive Psychology,19, 151–165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Chase, W. G., &Simon, H. A. (1973). The mind’s eye in chess. In W. G. Chase (Ed.),Visual information processing: Proceedings (pp. 215–281). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  11. de Groot, A. D. (1965).Thought and choice in chess. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
  12. Divinsky, N. (1990).The Batsford chess encyclopedia. London: Batsford.Google Scholar
  13. Elo, A. E. (1965). Age changes in master chess performances.Journal of Gerontology,20, 289–299.Google Scholar
  14. Elo, A. E. (1986).The rating of chessplayers, past and present (2nd ed.). New York: Arco.Google Scholar
  15. Frydman, M., &Lynn, R. (1992). The general intelligence and spatial abilities of gifted young Belgian chess players.British Journal of Psychology,83, 233–235.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Gaige, J. (1987).Chess personalia: A biobibliography. London: McFarland.Google Scholar
  17. Glickman, M. E. (1995). Chess rating systems.American Chess Journal,3, 59–102.Google Scholar
  18. Gobet, F., de Voogt, A. J., &Retschitzki, J. (2004).Moves in mind: The psychology of board games. Hove, U.K.: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  19. Graham, J. (1984).The literature of chess. Jefferson, NC: MacFarlane.Google Scholar
  20. Holding, D. H. (1985).The psychology of chess skill. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  21. Howard, R. W. (1995).Learning and memory: Major ideas, principles, issues, and applications. Westport, CT: Praeger.Google Scholar
  22. Howard, R. W. (1999). Preliminary real-world evidence that average human intelligence really is rising.Intelligence,27, 235–250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Howard, R. W. (2001). Searching the real world for signs of rising population intelligence.Personality & Individual Differences,30, 1039–1058.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Howard, R. W. (2004). A revealing picture of chess: 33 years of FIDE ratings and data compiled.Chess Life,59, 8–10.Google Scholar
  25. Howard, R. W. (2005a). Are gender differences in high achievement disappearing? A test in one intellectual domain.Journal of Biosocial Science,37, 371–380.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Howard, R. W. (2005b). Objective evidence of rising population ability: Detailed examination of longitudinal chess data.Personality & Individual Differences,38, 347–363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Mantanovic, A. (2000).Chess is chess [CD-ROM]. Budapest: Chess Informant.Google Scholar
  28. Milojkovic, J. D. (1982). Chess imagery in novice and master.Journal of Mental Imagery,6, 125–144.Google Scholar
  29. Newell, A. (1973). You can’t play 20 questions with nature and win. In W. G. Chase (Ed.),Visual information processing: Proceedings (pp. 283–308). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  30. Nunn, J. (1999).John Nunn’s chess puzzle book. London: Gambit.Google Scholar
  31. O’Connell, K. J. (1976).The Batsford chess yearbook, 1975/6. London: Batsford.Google Scholar
  32. Reitman, J. S. (1976). Skilled perception in go. Cognitive Psychology,8, 336–356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Saariluoma, P. (1991). Aspects of skilled imagery in blindfold chess.Acta Psychologica,77, 65–89.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Saariluoma, P. (1992). Visuospatial and articulatory interference in chess players’ information intake.Applied Cognitive Psychology,6, 77–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Schwarz, A. M., Schachinger, H., Adler, R. H., &Goetz, S. M. (2003). Hopelessness is associated with decreased heart rate variability during championship chess games.Psychosomatic Medicine,65, 658–661.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Sunnucks, A. (1976).The encyclopedia of chess. London: Robert Hale.Google Scholar
  37. van der Maas, H. L. J., &Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2005). A psychometric analysis of chess expertise.American Journal of Psychology,118, 29–60.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Waters, A. J., Gobet, F., &Leyden, G. (2002). Visuospatial abilities of chess players.British Journal of Psychology,93, 557–565.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Whyld, K. (1986).Chess: The records. Enfield, U.K.: Guinness.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of EducationUniversity of New South WalesSydneyAustralia

Personalised recommendations