Advertisement

Behavior Research Methods

, Volume 39, Issue 2, pp 248–258 | Cite as

DEWEX: A system for designing and conducting Web-based experiments

  • Anja NaumannEmail author
  • Angela Brunstein
  • Josef F. Krems
Articles From the SCiP Conference

Abstract

DEWEX is a server-based environment for developing Web-based experiments. It provides many features for creating and running complex experimental designs on a local server. It is freeware and allows for both using default features, for which only text input is necessary, and easy configurations that can be set up by the experimenter. The tool also provides log files on the local server that can be interpreted and analyzed very easily. As an illustration of how DEWEX can be used, a recent study is presented that demonstrates the system’s most important features. This study investigated learning from multiple hypertext sources and shows the influences of task, source of information, and hypertext presentation format on the construction of mental representations of a hypertext about a historical event.

Keywords

Radio Button Multiple Text Page Break Data Folder Experiment Folder 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Birnbaum, M. H. (2000). SurveyWiz and FactorWiz: JavaScript Web pages that make HTML forms for research on the Internet.Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers,32, 339–346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Britt, M. A., Rouet, J.-F., &Perfetti, C. A. (1996). Using hypertext to study and reason about historical evidence. In J.-F. Rouet, J. J. Levonen, A. Dillon, & R. J. Spiro (Eds.),Hypertext and cognition (pp. 43–72). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  3. Brunstein, A., Naumann, A., &Krems, J. F. (2005). The Chemnitz LogAnalyzer: A tool for analyzing data from hypertext navigation research.Behavior Research Methods,37, 232–239.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Gerdes, H. (1997).Lernen mit Text und Hypertext [Learning with text and hypertext]. Lengerich: Pabst.Google Scholar
  5. Hemmerich, J. A., &Wiley, J. (2002). Do argumentation tasks promote conceptual change about volcanoes? In W. D. Gray & C. Schunn (Eds.),Proceedings of the Twenty-Fourth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 453–458). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  6. Naumann, A. (2003).Wissenserwerb und Informationssuche mit Hypertexten: Die Bedeutung von Strukturierung, Navigationshilfen und Arbeitsgedächtnisbelastung [Knowledge acquisition and information retrieval with hypertext: The impact of structure, navigation aids and working memory load]. Doctoral dissertation, Technische Universität Chemnitz, Germany. Retrieved September 16, 2006, from archiv.tu-chemnitz.de/pub/2004/0117/index.html.Google Scholar
  7. Naumann, A., Waniek, J., &Krems, J. F. (2001). Knowledge acquisition, navigation, and eye movements from text and hypertext. In U.-D. Reips & M. Bosnjak (Eds.),Dimensions of Internet science (pp. 293–304). Lengerich: Pabst.Google Scholar
  8. Naumann, A., Wechsung, I., & Krems, J. F. (2006).Learning from multiple hypertext sources. Manuscript in preparation.Google Scholar
  9. Reips, U.-D. (2002a). Internet-based psychological experimenting: Five dos and five don’ts.Social Science Computer Review,20, 241–249.Google Scholar
  10. Reips, U.-D. (2002b). Standards for Internet-based experimenting.Experimental Psychology,49, 243–256.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Reips, U.-D., &Neuhaus, C. (2002). WEXTOR: A Web-based tool for generating and visualizing experimental designs and procedures.Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers,34, 234–240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Rouet, J.-F., Britt, M. A., Mason, R. A., &Perfetti, C. A. (1996). Using multiple sources of evidence to reason about history.Journal of Educational Psychology,88, 478–493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Schmidt, W. C. (2002). A server-side program for delivering experiments with animations.Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers,34, 208–217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Voss, J. F., &Van Dyke, J. A. (2001). Argumentation in psychology: Background comments.Discourse Processes,32, 89–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Voss, J. F., &Wiley, J. (2000). A case study of developing historical understanding via instruction: The importance of integrating text components and constructing arguments. In P. N. Stearns, P. Seixas, & S. Wineburg (Eds.),Knowing, teaching, and learning history: National and international perspectives (pp. 375–389). New York: NYU Press.Google Scholar
  16. Wiley, J. (2001). Supporting understanding through task and browser design. In J. D. Moore & K. Stenning (Eds.),Proceedings of the Twenty-Third Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1164–1169). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  17. Wiley, J., &Voss, J. F. (1996). The effects of “playing historian” on learning in history.Applied Cognitive Psychology,10(Spec. Issue), S63-S72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Wiley, J., &Voss, J. F. (1999). Constructing arguments from multiple sources: Tasks that promote understanding and not just memory for text.Journal of Educational Psychology,91, 301–311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • Anja Naumann
    • 1
    Email author
  • Angela Brunstein
    • 2
  • Josef F. Krems
    • 3
  1. 1.Deutsche Telekom LaboratoriesBernn University of TechnologyBerlinGermany
  2. 2.Carnegie Mellon UniversityPittsburghPennsylvania
  3. 3.Chemnitz University of TechnologyChemnitzGermany

Personalised recommendations