Abstract
Although many studies have looked at the perceptual—cognitive strategies used to make anticipatory judgments in sport, few have examined the informational invariants that our visual system may be attuned to. Using immersive interactive virtual reality to simulate the aerodynamics of the trajectory of a ball with and without sidespin, the present study examined the ability of expert and novice soccer players to make judgments about the ball’s future arrival position. An analysis of their judgment responses showed how participants were strongly influenced by the ball’s trajectory. The changes in trajectory caused by sidespin led to erroneous predictions about the ball’s future arrival position. An analysis of potential informational variables that could explain these results points to the use of a first-order compound variable combining optical expansion and optical displacement.
Article PDF
Similar content being viewed by others
Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.
References
Abernethy, B., Gill, D. P., Parks, S. L., & Packer, S. T. (2001). Expertise and the perception of kinematic and situational probability information. Perception, 30, 233–252.
Bootsma, R. J. (1991). Predictive information and the control of action: What you see is what you get. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 22, 271–278.
Bootsma, R. J., & Craig, C. M. (2002). Global and local contributions to the optical specification of time to contact: Observer sensitivity to composite tau. Perception, 31, 901–924.
Bootsma, R. J., Fayt, V., Zaal, F. T. J. M., & Laurent, M. (1997). On the information-based regulation of movement: What Wann (1996) may want to consider. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 23, 1282–1289.
Bray, K., & Kerwin, D. G. (2003). Modelling the flight of a soccer ball in a direct free kick. Journal of Sports Sciences, 21, 75–85.
Carré, M. J., Asai, T., Akatsuka, T., & Haake, S. J. (2002). The curve kick of a football: II. Flight through the air. Sports Engineering, 5, 193–200.
Craig, C. M., Berton, E., Rao, G., Fernandez, L., & Bootsma, R. J. (2006). Judging where a ball will go: The case of curved free kicks in football. Naturwissenschaften, 93, 97–101.
de Mestre, N. (1990). The mathematics of projectiles in sport. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Farrow, D., & Abernethy, B. (2003). Do expertise and the degree of perception—action coupling affect natural anticipatory performance? Perception, 32, 1127–1139.
Gabbiani, F., Krapp, H. G., Koch, C., & Laurent, G. (2002). Multiplicative computation in a visual neuron sensitive to looming. Nature, 420, 320–324.
Goulet, C., Bard, C., & Fleury, M. (1989). Expertise differences in preparing to return a tennis serve: A visual information processing approach. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 11, 382–398.
Gray, R., & Regan, D. M. (2006). Unconfounding the direction of motion in depth, time to passage and rotation rate of an approaching object. Vision Research, 46, 2388–2402.
Harris, J. M., & Drga, V. F. (2005). Using visual direction in threedimensional motion perception. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 229–233.
Lee, D. N. (1998). Guiding movement by coupling taus. Ecological Psychology, 10, 221–250.
Lee, D. N., & Young, D. S. (1985). Visual timing of interceptive action. In D. J. Ingle, M. Jeannerod, & D. N. Lee (Eds.), Brain mechanisms and spatial vision (pp. 1–30). Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhoff.
McLeod, P., & Dienes, Z. (1996). Do fielders know where to go to catch the ball or only how to get there? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 22, 531–543.
Mehta, R. D. (1985). Aerodynamics of sports balls. Annual Review of Fluid Mechanics, 17, 151–189.
Michaels, C. F., & Oudejans, R. R. D. (1992). The optics and actions of catching fly balls: Zeroing out optical acceleration. Ecological Psychology, 4, 199–222.
Montagne, G., Laurent, M., Durey, A., & Bootsma, R. [J.] (1999). Movement reversals in ball catching. Experimental Brain Research, 129, 87–92.
Morya, E., Ranvaud, R., & Pinheiro, W. M. (2003). Dynamics of visual feedback in a laboratory simulation of a penalty kick. Journal of Sport Sciences, 21, 87–95.
Oudejans, R. R. D., Michaels, C. F., Bakker, F. C., & Dolné, M. A. (1996). The relevance of action in perceiving affordances: Perception of catchableness of fly balls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 22, 879–891.
Peper, L., Bootsma, R. J., Mestre, D. R., & Bakker F. C. (1994). Catching balls: How to get the hand to the right place at the right time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 20, 591–612.
Regan, D. [M.], & Kaushal, S. (1994). Monocular discrimination of the direction of motion in depth. Vision Research, 34, 163–177.
Shaffer, D. M., McBeath, M. K., Roy, W. L., & Krauchunas, S. M. (2003). A linear optical trajectory informs the fielder where to run to the side to catch fly balls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 29, 1244–1250.
Sun, H., & Frost, B. J. (1998). Computation of different optical variables of looming objects in pigeon nucleus rotundus neurons. Nature Neuroscience, 1, 296–303.
Williams, A. M., & Ericsson, K. A. (2005). Perceptual—cognitive expertise in sport: Some considerations when applying the expert performance approach. Human Movement Science, 24, 283–307.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
Note—Accepted by the previous editorial team, when Thomas H. Carr was Editor.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Craig, C.M., Goulon, C., Berton, E. et al. Optic variables used to judge future ball arrival position in expert and novice soccer players. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 71, 515–522 (2009). https://doi.org/10.3758/APP.71.3.515
Received:
Accepted:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/APP.71.3.515