Measurement of saccadic eye movements by electrooculography for simultaneous EEG recording
Abstract
Eye movements are an important index of the neural functions of visual information processing, decision making, visuomotor coordination, sports performance, and so forth. However, the available optical tracking methods are impractical in many situations, such as the wearing of eyeglasses or the presence of ophthalmic disorders, and this can be overcome by accurate recording of eye movements by electrooculography (EOG). In this study we recorded eye movements by EOG simultaneously with high-density electroencephalogram (EEG) recording using a 128-channel EGI electrode net at a 500-Hz sampling rate, including appropriate facial electrodes. The participants made eye movements over a calibration target consisting of a 5×5 grid of stimulus targets. The results showed that the EOG methodology allowed accurate analysis of the amplitude and direction of the fixation locations and saccadic dynamics with a temporal resolution of 500 Hz, under both cued and uncued analysis regimes. Blink responses could be identified separately and were shown to have a more complex source derivation than has previously been recognized. The results also showed that the EOG signals recorded through the EEG net can achieve results as accurate as typical optical eye-tracking devices, and also allow for simultaneous assessment of neural activity during all types of eye movements. Moreover, the EOG method effectively avoids the technical difficulties related to eye-tracker positioning and the synchronization between EEG and eye movements. We showed that simultaneous EOG/EEG recording is a convenient means of measuring eye movements, with an accuracy comparable to that of many specialized eye-tracking systems.
Keywords
Saccades Eye movements EOG Free-viewing Blink detectionA wide variety of human brain functions have been studied with event-related analyses, in the form of averaged event-related potentials (ERPs). In the vast majority of those experiments, visual processing has been examined with large eye movements precluded, by asking participants to hold their gaze still in order to maintain fixation, and to refrain from blinking while the stimuli are flashed near the fovea. To further prohibit saccadic eye movements, visual stimuli are often presented for a short duration within the saccadic reaction time or at a small stimulus size.
However, the practice of precluding oculomotor activity in most electroencephalographic (EEG) studies eliminates a major component of visual information processing. It has long been established that the eyes produce a continual stream of saccadic eye movements to bring the fovea to bear on aspects of interest in the visual scene. In addition, confining experiments to conditions without eye movements cannot elucidate the cortical mechanisms involved in control of the eye movements themselves.
To investigate the brain activity specific to eye movement control, we need to measure brain responses through EEG signals and eye movements simultaneously. It has been justified that combining EEG and eye tracking is possible (Nikolaev, Meghanathan, & van Leeuwen, 2016). Currently, there are many different methods to measure eye movements, with varying difficulties and costs. One option is infrared video-based eye tracking systems, which can achieve good accuracy. But concurrent eye tracking in the process of measuring EEG signals requires the management of technical issue related to synchronization. Special trigger pulses (Baccino & Manunta, 2005) need to be sent to both the EEG and eye-tracking systems to achieve synchronization. Such eye trackers can be difficult to set up for certain individuals, particularly those needing to wear spectacles or low-vision aids for optimal vision.
Another option to measure saccadic eye movements is electric potential measurement by means of electrooculography (EOG). This reliable technique for measuring saccadic eye movements is based on the mechanism that the eyes are the origin of a steady potential field that can be modeled as a dipole with its positive pole at the cornea and negative pole at the retina. Electrodes placed in the periocular region are used to measure these electric potentials. If the eyes move from the center position to a peripheral location, the cornea approaches one electrode while the retina approaches the opposing one. This change in the orientation of the dipole results in a change in the measured EOG signal. The EOG technique has the advantage in concurrent eye tracking that the simultaneous measurements of EEG signals and eye movements by EOG can be achieved by using the same high-density electrode net. This effectively avoids the technical difficulties related to eye-tracker positioning and synchronization between EEG and eye movements.
Some previous algorithms have been developed for the detection of saccades in EOG. Niemenlehto (2009) employed a detection approach of constant false alarm rate to detect saccades during the analysis of EOG signals. Pettersson et al. (2013) used the temporal derivative of the EOG eye movement signals for determining the threshold to detect saccades and blinks. Behrens, MacKeben, and Schröder-Preikschat (2010) utilized the deviation of eye-movement acceleration values from the EOG signals. Toivanen, Pettersson, and Lukander (2015) considered unsupervised training to establish a real-time probabilistic algorithm for detecting saccades. However, none of these studies systematically measured the saccades beyond the central 10° field using seven eye movement EOG electrodes, or assessed the linearity of the saccade trajectories, or estimated the bivariate error function of their measurements of the resulting saccade endpoints in x, y space, or compared them with the errors reported for other techniques.
This present study was designed to assess the efficacy EOG signals measured from the high-density EEG electrode net for the accurate analysis of the amplitudes and directions of saccades. This article also provides a sophisticated analysis of the nature of blink responses through detailed measures of EOG signals around eyes.
Method
Participants
We recruited seven participants for the simultaneous measurement of EEG and EOG (five male, two female; age range: 27–37 years), all with normal or corrected-to-normal vision. They were each informed about the purpose of the study and the procedures, and had provided a written consent before the measurements. All procedures conformed to the Declaration of Helsinki and were approved by the Smith-Kettlewell Institutional Review Board.
Stimulus presentation
The stimuli were presented on a 19-in. color monitor (Sony LCD Color Monitor, MODEL: SDM-S93, Sony Electronics, Inc. FL, refresh rate 100 Hz, resolution 1,280 × 1,024 pixels) at a viewing distance of 33 cm. A 5×5 stimulus array was configured in a square region of 34° × 34°, subtended in the center of a dark screen with a background luminance of 3.06 cd/m2. A red target (size ~ 0.5°) appeared randomly within this square grid at 25 possible stimulus locations.
Experimental procedures
The experimental paradigms (see the text for details)
EEG recordings
EEG data were recorded using an EGI Geodesic Netstation high-density, whole-head recording system (Electrical Geodesics, Inc., Eugene, OR), which incorporates 128 electrodes distributed around the head and face, in addition to the scalp electrodes. The impedance of all electrodes was maintained below 60 kΩ according to the recommended value for this system. The data were recorded at a sampling rate of 500 Hz and band-pass filtered between 0.01 and 250 Hz. The recorded signals for all electrodes were referenced to the vertex electrode (Cz).
Eye tracking
Eye movements were recorded through the measurement of electric potentials (EOG) using the Netstation electrodes located around the eyes, which included channels appropriate for recording vertical and horizontal EOGs. The vertical EOG for each eye was derived from the differential signal between electrodes located in the infraorbital ridge of the eye and the one immediately above the eye (#126 and #8 for the right eye, and #127 and #25 for the left eye in the Netstation coding), and the horizontal EOG was derived from the differential signal between electrodes located in the middle of both eyes (#17) and the one near the outer canthus of each eye (#1 for the right eye and #32 for the left eye).
Preprocessing and analysis
All recorded EEG data were preprocessed and analyzed in Matlab (Mathworks, Natick, MA). Electrical mains 60 Hz and monitor refresh 100-Hz pickup were first removed by an innovative frequency splatter filtering procedure (Tyler & Likova, 2017). Then, EEG data were low-pass filtered at 100 Hz and re-referenced to the average activity of the 126 scalp electrodes. The signals from two electrodes near each ear (#113 near the right ear, and #49 near the left ear) were eliminated due to a high noise level. Two kinds of analyses were performed. One used the stimuli as the time markers, and the other analyzed the eye-movement waveforms as if they were from free-viewing condition, without reference to the time markers. For the saccade detection with a target cue, the recordings were segmented into 1.5 s epochs from – 500 to + 1,000 ms around the onset of each target. The data in each epoch were then high-pass filtered at 0.2 Hz to remove low frequency drift, and the horizontal and vertical EOG signals in each epoch were each baseline-corrected to the level of the stimulus interval of 100 ms just preceding the stimulus. The vector sum of the baseline-corrected horizontal and vertical EOG signal amplitude was used to detect saccades.
Cued saccade detection
The custom software for saccade detection with a target cue operates as follows. A recording of combined EOG signals of horizontal and vertical EOG were processed continually throughout each epoch. The onset of saccades was specified analytically as any point of significant deviation from the local noise level, as defined by the running average of the standard deviation (RASD) of the combined EOG signal amplitude over the previous 500 ms (Behrens et al., 2010). This principle was implemented as the point at which combined EOG signal amplitude first exceeded a criterion level of 5 × RASD. Specifically, the RASD was obtained by segmenting each 500-ms period into 50-ms-long overlapping data epochs, advancing in 25-ms steps from – 500 to 0 ms, where 0 is the onset of a target. Thus, there were 19 overlapping windows with 19 standard deviations during this 500-ms pretarget period. The RASD was then computed as the average of the mean of the lowest ten standard deviations (SDs) over all the artifact-free epochs. To define the saccade onset, the criterion time point for the detected saccade was backdated to a time point of 25 samples (50 ms) before the currently inspected point. Once the onset of the saccade was found, the EOG data were resegmented into the range from – 400 to 400 ms relative to the saccade onset for the detection of saccade offsets, as follows: The offsets of saccades were obtained through the velocity function of the normalized vector sum of the horizontal and vertical EOG signal amplitudes from – 400 to 400 ms relative to the onset of saccade. The saccade offset was then defined as the first point at which the value of the velocity function fell to less than 15% of its maximum velocity.
Uncued saccade detection
Saccades could also be detected without applying the target cues, as a test of the capability of detecting uncued saccades, corresponding to free-viewing conditions. To assess the ability to identify the saccades without cueing, this uncued saccade detection procedure was based on processed the continuous horizontal and vertical EOG recorded for the entire experimental block (~ 2.5 min), without respect to the target presentation. First, the whole block was divided into intervals of 100 samples (200 ms). The presence of a saccade was assumed to generate an increase in the SD. All pairs of adjacent artifact-free EOG intervals, the second with a relatively higher SD than the first, were then identified throughout the recordings for the saccade detection analysis. For all samples in the second interval of the pair, the onset of saccades was then defined analytically as any sample point whose value deviated a criterion level of 5 × SD from the average SD in the 200-ms period immediately preceding that sample. The whole horizontal and vertical EOG sequences were probed separately in this way until all the specified saccades were detected, regardless of direction. This algorithm is applicable under any free-viewing conditions.
The effectiveness of the uncued procedure was assessed in terms of its sensitivity and specificity. Sensitivity was specified as the proportion of saccades detected with this uncued procedure in an experimental run that actually contained cued saccades to provide the ground truth for the saccade occurrence. Specificity was defined as the proportion of correct rejections provided by the same procedure applied to intertarget intervals that did not contain saccade (i.e., from at least 500 ms after the preceding target to just before the next target).
Calibration procedure
where bv and cv is a set of slope and intercept scaling factors for this vertical affine scaling function. With regard to the stability of the calibration over time, these affine calibration expressions were assumed to be specific to the electrode locations. Therefore, in common with typical eye movement calibration procedures, the affine transformation was invariant over time once established for a given set of electrode placements.
Principal component analysis (PCA)
To identify eye movement control components from the data, we performed an iterative form of PCA on the matrix of saccade-defined epochs to avoid the orthogonality constraint, which is unlikely to represent the physiological organization of the EEG responses. PCA in general is a blind source separation algorithm that enables the separation of statistically distinguishable sources from multidimensional data, and was implemented using the singular value decomposition (svd) function in MATLAB. In its full implementation, svd derives the set of n orthogonal vectors characterizing the input EEG data matrix, where n is the number of parallel signal vector inputs (matrix rows). To distinguish the directions of the saccades for the PCA analyses, all the saccades were separated into four direction sectors according to their closest adjacent cardinal direction (left, up, right and down) before applying the PCA. For each participant individually, the EEG amplitudes were averaged in each sector, yielding four 2-D time-by-electrode matrices. To get each principal component, the four two-dimensional (2-D) time-by-electrode matrices for the four conditions were concatenated in the second (electrode) dimension to get an omnibus 2-D matrix. A full svd on this omnibus 2-D matrix yielded orthogonal temporal component waveforms and spatial distributions. Only the primary temporal component (eigenvector) and spatial distribution were retained from the first svd of the concatenated the data, the rest of the temporal components obtained being discarded. This first eigenvector, weighted by its eigenvalue, was then subtracted from all the data and a second svd was implemented on the remaining data to yield further temporal PCA component waveforms and spatial distributions. Again, only the first primary temporal component and spatial distribution were retained to form the second iterative principal component. A series of sequential svd was performed, each on a remaining data, until eight iterative components were reached. This whole process was called iterative PCA (iPCA). It had the advantage over the classic single PCA that the constraint of orthogonality among the produced principal components by a single PCA was eliminated by the series of iterative PCA, thus providing the capability of revealing more physiological components of the eye movements and cortical process. In addition, multiple secondary peaks in an extracted component waveform might be eliminated through the iterations of PCA, which is easier for component interpretation.
Results
Blink artifact
(A) Illustration of the positions of the face electrodes, with numbering from the 128-electrode EGI net. (B) Blink potentials from one participant who was required to blink as fast as possible, resulting in approximately 8-Hz oscillations
The origin of the EOG signal
Event-related potentials during left, right, upward, and downward saccades with different amplitudes, as measured at (A) the left temple electrode and (B) the right temple electrode for one participant during the overlap paradigm. The responses are aligned to the onset of the stimulus at time 0
The horizontal and vertical electrooculography (EOG) signals for one participant during the overlap paradigm are aligned to the onset of the stimulus at time 0. The horizontal EOG (A) is obtained from subtraction between the EOGs recorded from the electrodes placed at the right and left temples, whereas the vertical EOG (B) is obtained from subtraction between the mean EOGs recorded from the electrodes placed above and below both eyes
Detection of saccades using EOG with a target cue
Vector-combined electrooculography (EOG) signals, with the computed saccade onsets aligned at time 0 for (A) the overlap paradigm and (B) the gap paradigm for one participant.
We also noticed that there was a presaccadic blip in the EOG signals (Harrison, Mattingley, & Remington, 2012; Klostermann et al., 1994), which might be due to receptive fields shifting to the next location in the stimulus grid prior to the onset of a saccade.
Saccade detection under free-viewing conditions
Horizontal (top) and vertical (bottom) electrooculography (EOG) signals, with saccade onset times detected by the free-viewing detection algorithm marked by ticks, for an experimental block in the overlap paradigm
Calibration
Parameters obtained in the calibration procedure, separately for both the horizontal and vertical dimensions, for (A) the overlap paradigm and (B) the gap paradigm
As is shown, in the overlap paradigm, the calculated slope and intercept in the horizontal prediction (parameters ah and ch in Eq. 2) were 5.984 μV/deg and 1.239 μV, respectively, whereas the slope and the intercept in the vertical prediction (parameters bv and cv in Eq. 3) were – 7.693 μV/deg and 9.620 μV, respectively. The coefficients of determination, R2, for the horizontal and vertical predictions are .994 and .884, respectively, with residual standard deviations of 0.318% and 5.981%.
These fits reveal that, following affine transformation, there is a highly linear relationship between the target locations and the saccade amplitudes predicted from the EOG signals horizontally, and to a lesser degree vertically.
where ah and ch are the dominant slope and intercept of the three-parameter affine scaling function for the horizontal prediction, and bv and cv are those for the vertical prediction.
Calibrated saccade trajectories for each cardinal direction for (A) the overlap paradigm and (B) the gap paradigm
Errors of calibrated saccade endpoints around each target location for (A) the overlap paradigm and (B) the gap paradigm
Heat maps of net errors of eye gaze calibration using the electrooculography (EOG) signals for (A) the overlap paradigm and (B) the gap paradigm
EEG analysis using PCA
Eight iterative components obtained from an iterative form of principal component analysis
Discussion
The novel approach to the accurate recording of eye movements developed in this article is to record EOG simultaneously with scalp EEG recording, to provide an accurate and convenient means of eye tracking without specialized eye-tracking equipment. Two interesting issues are discussed here. One is to compare the accuracy and linearity of EEG/EOG measurement with those of conventional video-based eye-tracking systems. The other is to consider the nature of the blink response measured by the EOG.
General utility of the EEG/EOG approach to eye tracking
There is a general trend toward combining eye-tracking capability with other types of brain imaging, such as functional magnetic resonance imaging and EEG. The use of dedicated eye trackers in this situation is both cumbersome and unreliable, since they can take a while to set up and do not work for all eye configurations. Moreover, they are particularly challenging for use with those who wear spectacles or low-vision aids. In the case of EEG recording of brain activity, it is greatly advantageous to have a technique for recording eye movements that does not require the use of additional equipment, but is derived directly from the same EEG electrode net as the brain signals. The combined EEG/EOG approach will work for any eye, regardless of color or optical quality, and in combination of the regular refractive devices or optical aids.
Comparison of EOG and video-based tracking of eye movements
As the calibrated saccadic direction vector results show, the EOG signals recorded simultaneously with EEG by using the high-density electrode net can attain results as accurate as many video-based eye tracking devices. The average angular accuracy reported across an array of detection techniques used for various types of eye tracking was 1.56° ± 0.23° (Hansen & Ji, 2010). In comparison, the present mean residual errors across the current stimulus grid for both the overlap and gap paradigms measured by EOG signals are 0.78° ± 0.03° and 1.49° ± 0.11° in the horizontal and vertical directions, respectively. These error ranges are thus in the lower end of those of typical eye-tracking technology.
The better performance for the horizontal EOG measurements in comparison with vertical EOG signals confirms a previous EOG study (Acuña, Aqueveque, & Pino, 2014). The recorded EOG potentials calibrated into gaze angle in degrees was more accurate for the horizontal direction than for the vertical direction. The calibration results, shown in Fig. 8, also indicated more dispersion in the vertical EOG signals, suggesting more variability than the corresponding horizontal EOG signals. One factor that might account for this greater variability is movement of the eyelid relative to the eyeball during saccades, which, as we showed in the first section, can generate substantial potentials independent of the eyeball rotation per se that could add noise to the vertical (but not the horizontal) EOG signal. In addition, there may be an effect of the more active muscles located in the eyebrows and the cheeks where the vertical EOG electrodes were placed, affecting the vertical EOG signals.
The data also show that both horizontal and vertical EOG signals provide excellent linearity up to at least 17°, since the extension in the stimulus grid reached to 17° in each of four cardinal directions. This is consistent with Acuña et al. (2014), who reported linearity up to 50°, which is a better linearity than video-based eye-tracking systems, with linearity only up to 25° (Bahill, Clark, & Stark, 1975).
As we mentioned in the introduction, the EOG technique used in this study has particular advantages as compared with video-based methods in the measurement of eye movements. The EOG system can be used with any kind of eyewear, including regular spectacles and low-vision aids. In addition, it does not require a fixed head position, allowing the investigation of natural gaze behavior, to the extent that the EOG recording system allows free head movements and is portable.
Blink response
Although some previous studies have shown that blink artifacts are caused by the cornea being insulated from the peri-ocular regions (Faes, van der Meij, de Munck, & Heethaar, 1999; Fish & Geddes, 2009), others have suggested that the eyelids act as a conductor during blinks (Antervo, Hari, Katila, Ryhänen, & Seppänen, 1985; Matsuo, Peters, & Reilly, 1975), allowing the positive corneal potentials to be conducted to the extra-ocular regions. If the insulator hypothesis for blink potentials were valid, the eyelids would block transmission of the positively charged corneal potential to the scalp, and the blink potentials at all EOG electrode sites should therefore be negative. On the other hand, if the conduction hypothesis were true, the eyelids would conduct the positive flow from the cornea to the extraocular skin, thus generating positive blink potentials at all EOG electrode sites.
However, although our EOG measurements are generally in support of the conduction hypothesis, detailed measures at various EOG electrode sites around the eyes indicate a different scenario. The blink potentials are strongly positive for the forehead electrode derivations, but weakly negative for the infraorbital derivations, and close to zero for the canthi derivations. We conclude that neither the volume conduction nor the insulator hypothesis can fully explain these results, although the sign of the larger forehead signal is consistent with the conduction hypothesis related to conduction through the upper eyelid.
One possible explanation for these results is that the assumption that the eyes are stationary during rapid blinks is incorrect. In principle, if the eyes moved upward to a sufficient extent during the blinks, they could invert the infraorbital signal while more than doubling the forehead signal. According to the calibration data of Fig. 7, such rapid-blink signals would have required an upward eye movement of about 10° (corresponding to a differential signal between the forehead and cheek electrodes of about 160 μV). In the data from Fig. 2, however, the blink signals are as much as 500 μV, implying eye rotations of up to ~ 30° during free blinks.
There are three lines of reasoning against this interpretation, however.
The participant in the blink control study in Fig. 2 was specifically instructed to maintain fixation while performing the rapid blink sequence, and reported no visible movement of the fixation target. Since the eyelids are open to the central target region for most of the lid movement, until just before it closes, it should have been easy for the participant to see any corresponding retinal motion produced by the large eye movements required for this explanation.
Riggs, Kelly, Manning, and Moore (1987, p.334) found that the eyes move very little during blinks when they are close to the primary straight-ahead position: “In normal conditions of viewing there is no evidence of conjugate saccades, or of any large, upward rotation of the eyes (Bell’s phenomenon) that was once believed to take place during a blink.” (When converged, the eyes tend to move back toward the primary position during blinks, but this is a horizontal rather than a vertical movement, and is thus inconsistent with all our recorded blink signals in the vertically aligned electrodes.) In detail, the Riggs et al. results show that the minimum blink motion for a fixation position slightly nasalward and upward of primary position, but the motion in primary position itself was only about 1°, so it would be drastically insufficient to account for our results with the eyes in primary position.
According to the difference in the time courses of the eye rotation signals and the more rapid blink signals reported by Riggs et al. (1987), the blink responses should appear as a complex difference in the waveforms recorded above and below the eyes during blinks, but no such differences are apparent in Fig. 2.
Given this analysis, the only plausible explanation for the variations in sign of the eye blink responses is that there are differential insulation/conduction relationships according to the composition of the eyelids. If the skin operates as an electrical insulator, it could account for the reduction in the cheek signal with closure of the lower lid. If, on the other hand, the levator muscle underlying the upper lid acted as an electrical conductor, it could account for the large increase in the forehead signal, sufficient to overcome any insulator effects. We conclude that the muscle acts as an electrical conductor and the skin acts as an electrical insulator to produce the variety of blink responses that we see from the facial electrode derivations.
Conclusions
The present eye-tracking capability using EOG signals from a high-density electrode net verifies that EOG can be used for simultaneous measurements of brain responses and eye movements. This capability will be particularly useful for measuring fixation- or saccade-related EEG potentials in order to examine the neural mechanisms involved in the control of eye movements, without the difficulties of synchronization between separate EEG and video-based eye-tracking devices. Further studies aiming to determine fixation- or saccade-related potentials under free-viewing conditions could be based on the present results, further validating the accuracy of calibrated EOG signals for 2-D eye tracking.
Notes
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by a Rachel C. Atkinson fellowship from Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute. Thanks to Spero Nicholas for programming the EEG filter and for EEG preprocessing.
Author contribution
Y.J. and C.W.T. designed the study, Y.J. collected the data, and Y.J. and C.W.T. developed the data analysis and wrote the article.
Compliance with ethical standards
Conflict of interest
Neither of the authors have potential conflicts of interest to be disclosed.
References
- Acuña, O. V., Aqueveque, P., & Pino, E. J. (2014). Eye-tracking capabilities of low-cost EOG system. Article presented at the Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), 2014 36th Annual International Conference of the IEEE, Chicago, IL.Google Scholar
- Antervo, A., Hari, R., Katila, T., Ryhänen, T., & Seppänen, M. (1985). Magnetic fields produced by eye blinking. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 61, 247–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Baccino, T., & Manunta, Y. (2005). Eye-fixation-related potentials: Insight into parafoveal processing. Journal of Psychophysiology, 19, 204–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bahill, A. T., Clark, M. R., & Stark, L. (1975). The main sequence, a tool for studying human eye movements. Mathematical Biosciences, 24, 191–204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Behrens, F., MacKeben, M., & Schröder-Preikschat, W. (2010). An improved algorithm for automatic detection of saccades in eye movement data and for calculating saccade parameters. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 701–708. doi: https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.42.3.701 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Clark, J. J. (1999). Spatial attention and latencies of saccadic eye movements. Vision Research, 39, 585–602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Faes, T. J., van der Meij, H. A., de Munck, J. C., & Heethaar, R. M. (1999). The electric resistivity of human tissues (100 Hz–10 MHz): A meta-analysis of review studies. Physiological Measurement, 20, R1–R10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Fish, R. M., & Geddes, L. A. (2009). Conduction of electrical present to and through the human body: A review. Eplasty, 9, e44.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Hansen, D. W., & Ji, Q. (2010). In the eye of the beholder: A survey of models for eyes and gaze. IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence, 32, 478–500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Harrison, W. J., Mattingley, J. B., & Remington, R. W. (2012). Pre-saccadic shifts of visual attention. PLoS ONE, 7, e45670. doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0045670 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Klostermann, W., Kömpf, D., Heide, W., Verleger, R., Wauschkuhn, B., & Seyfert, T. (1994). The presaccadic cortical negativity prior to self-paced saccades with and without visual guidance. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 91, 219–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Matsuo, F., Peters, J. F., & Reilly, E. L. (1975). Electrical phenomena associated with movements of the eyelid. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 38, 507–511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Niemenlehto, P.-H. (2009). Constant false alarm rate detection of saccadic eye movements in electro-oculography. Computer Methods and Programs in Biomedicine, 96, 158–171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Nikolaev, A. R., Meghanathan, R. N., & van Leeuwen, C. (2016). Combining EEG and eye movement recording in free viewing: Pitfalls and possibilities. Brain and Cognition, 107, 55–83. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2016.06.004 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Pettersson, K., Jagadeesan, S., Lukander, K., Henelius, A., Hæggström, E., & Müller, K. (2013). Algorithm for automatic analysis of electro-oculographic data. Biomedical Engineering Online, 12, 110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Riggs, L. A., Kelly, J. P., Manning, K. A., & Moore, R. K. (1987). Blink-related eye movements. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 28, 334–342.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Saslow, M. (1967). Effects of components of displacement-step stimuli upon latency for saccadic eye movement. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 57, 1024–1029.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Toivanen, M., Pettersson, K., & Lukander, K. (2015). A probabilistic real-time algorithm for detecting blinks, saccades, and fixations from EOG data. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 8(2), 1. doi:10.16910/jemr.8.2.1Google Scholar
- Tyler, C. W., & Likova, L. T. (2017). Studying the retinal source of photophobia by facial electroretinography. Optometry and Vision Science, 94, 511–518. doi: https://doi.org/10.1097/OPX.0000000000001064 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
Copyright information
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.