Advertisement

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

, Volume 26, Issue 6, pp 1747–1766 | Cite as

Embodied memories: Reviewing the role of the body in memory processes

  • Francesco IanìEmail author
Theoretical Review

Abstract

This review aims at exploring the role of the body and its sensorimotor processes in memory. Recent theories have suggested that memories can profitably be seen as mental simulations consisting in the reactivation of sensorimotor patterns originally associated with events at encoding, rather than amodal mental representations. The sensorimotor model of memory (SMM) claims that the body is the medium where (and through which) sensorimotor modalities actually simulate the somatosensory components of remembered events, and predicts that memory processes can be manipulated through manipulation of the body. The review analyzes experimental evidence in favor of the SMM and the claim that the body is at stake in memory processes. The review then highlights how, at the current state of research, the majority of this evidence concerns the effect of body manipulations on memory processes rather than memory representations. It considers the need for a more circumstantial outline of the actual extent to which the body is capable of affecting memory, specifically on some important areas still unexplored, such as the sense of recollection. Resulting strengths and limitations of the SMM are discussed in relation to the more general debate on the embodied cognition.

Keywords

Memory Body Embodied cognition Sensorimotor reactivation Recollection 

Notes

References

  1. Arminjon, M., Preissmann, D., Chmetz, F., Duraku, A., Ansermet, F., & Magistretti, P. J. (2015). Embodied memory: Unconscious smiling modulates emotional evaluation of episodic memories. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 650.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00650 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  2. Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In K. W Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.), Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 2, pp. 89-195). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  3. Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–660.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Barsalou, L. W. (2009). Simulation, situated conceptualization, and prediction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 1281–1289.Google Scholar
  6. Barsalou, L. W., Breazeal, C., & Smith, L. B. (2007). Cognition as coordinated non-cognition. Cognitive Processing, 8, 79–91.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Barsalou, L. W., Santos, A., Simmons, W. K., & Wilson, C. D. (2008). Language and simulation in conceptual processing. In M. De Vega, A. M. Glemberg, & A. Graesser (Eds.). Symbols, embodiment, and meaning (pp. 245–283). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  8. Bietti, L. M. (2012). Towards a cognitive pragmatics of collective remembering. Pragmatics & Cognition, 20, 32–61.Google Scholar
  9. Binder, J. R., & Desai, R. H. (2011). The neurobiology of semantic memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15, 527–536.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  10. Barkow J., Cosmides L., & Tooby J. (Eds.). (1992). The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  11. Brunel, L., Labeye, E., Lesourd, M., & Versace, R. (2009). The sensory nature of episodic memory: Sensory priming effects due to memory trace activation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 1081–1088.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Seitz, R. J., … Freund, H. J. (2001). Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in somatotopic manner: An fMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 400–404.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Buss, D. M. (Ed.). (2005). The handbook of evolutionary psychology. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
  14. Caldwell, J. A., Prazinko, B., & Caldwell, J. L. (2003). Body posture affects electroencephalographic activity and psychomotor vigilance task performance in sleep-deprived subjects. Clinical Neurophysiology, 114, 23–31.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Callebaut, W., Rasskin-Gutman, D., & Simon, H. A. (Eds.). (2005). Modularity: Understanding the development and evolution of natural complex systems. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  16. Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grèzes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2004). Action observation and acquired motor skills: An FMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1243–1249.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Canales, J. Z., Cordás, T. A., Fiquer, J. T., Cavalcante, A. F., & Moreno, R. A. (2010). Posture and body image in individuals with major depressive disorder: A controlled study. Brazilian Journal of Psychiatry, 32, 375–380.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Canits, I., Pecher, D., & Zeelenberg, R. (2018). Effects of grasp compatibility on long-term memory for objects. Acta Psychologica, 182, 65–74.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Cardellicchio, P., Sinigaglia, C., & Costantini, M. (2011). The space of affordances: A TMS study. Neuropsychologia, 49, 1369–1372.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Carlini, A., Actis-Grosso, R., Stucchi, N., & Pozzo, T. (2012). Forward to the past. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 174.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  21. Carney, D. R., Cuddy, A. J., & Yap, A. J. (2010). Power posing: Brief nonverbal displays affect neuroendocrine levels and risk tolerance. Psychological science, 21, 1363-1368.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Casasanto, D., & Dijkstra, K. (2010). Motor action and emotional memory. Cognition, 115, 179–185.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  23. Casile, A., & Giese, M. A. (2006). Nonvisual motor training influences biological motion perception. Current Biology, 16, 69–74.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Chemero, A. (2009). Radical embodied cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  25. Chu, M., Meyer, A., Foulkes, L., & Kita, S. (2014). Individual differences in frequency and saliency of speech-accompanying gestures: The role of cognitive abilities and empathy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 694–709.Google Scholar
  26. Clark, A. (2007). Curing cognitive hiccups: A defense of the extended mind. The Journal of Philosophy, 104, 163–192.Google Scholar
  27. Clark, A., Nash, R. A., Fincham, G., & Mazzoni, G. (2012). Creating non-believed memories for recent autobiographical events. PLOS ONE, 7, e32998.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  28. Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82(6), 407–428.Google Scholar
  29. Conca, F., & Tettamanti, M. (2018). Conceptual semantics as grounded in personal experience. Phenomenology and Mind, 14, 98–116. doi:10.13128/Phe_Mi-23628Google Scholar
  30. Cook, S. W., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2009). Embodied communication: Speakers’ gestures affect listeners’ actions. Cognition, 113, 98–104.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  31. Cook, S. W., Yip, T. K., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2010). Gesturing makes memories that last. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 465–475.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  32. Cutica, I., & Bucciarelli, M. (2008). The deep versus the shallow: Effects of co-speech gestures in learning from discourse. Cognitive Science, 32, 921–935.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Cutica, I., Ianì, F., & Bucciarelli, M. (2014). Learning from text benefits from enactment. Memory & Cognition, 42, 1026–1037.Google Scholar
  34. Damasio, A. R. (1989). Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: A systems-level proposal for the neural substrates of recall and recognition. Cognition, 33, 25–62.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error. New York, NY: Avon Books.Google Scholar
  36. Damasio, A. R. (1996). The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 351, 1413–1420.Google Scholar
  37. D’Argembeau, A., & Van der Linden, M. (2006). Individual differences in the phenomenology of mental time travel: The effect of vivid visual imagery and emotion regulation strategies. Consciousness and Cognition, 15, 342–350.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Dewhurst, S. A., & Knott, L. M. (2010). Investigating the encoding—Retrieval match in recognition memory: Effects of experimental design, specificity, and retention interval. Memory & Cognition, 38, 1101–1109.Google Scholar
  39. Di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor events: A neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research, 91, 176–180.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Dijkstra, K., Kaschak, M. P., & Zwaan, R. A. (2007). Body posture facilitates retrieval of autobiographical memories. Cognition, 102, 139–149.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Dijkstra, K., & Post, L. (2015). Mechanisms of embodiment. Frontiers in Psychology.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01525
  42. Dijkstra, K., & Zwaan, R. A. (2014). Memory and action. In L. A. Shapiro (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of embodied cognition (pp. 296–305). Abingdon, England: Taylor & Francis Books.Google Scholar
  43. Earles, J. L., & Kersten, A. W. (2002). Directed forgetting of actions by younger and older adults. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 383–388.Google Scholar
  44. Edelman, G. (1987). Neural Darwinism: The theory of neuronal group selection. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  45. Edelman, G. (1989). The remembered present: A biological theory of consciousness. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  46. Eichenbaum, H., & Cohen N. J. (2001). From conditioning to conscious recollection: Memory systems of the brain. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  47. Engelkamp, J., & Zimmer, H. D. (1985). Motor programs and their relation to semantic memory. German Journal of Psychology, 9, 239–254.Google Scholar
  48. Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  49. Fodor, J. A., & Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1988). Connectionism and cognitive architecture: A critical analysis. Cognition, 28, 3–71.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Gallese, V. (2005). Being like me: Self–other identity, mirror neurons and empathy. In S. Hurley & N. Chater (Eds.), Perspectives on imitation: From cognitive neuroscience to social science. Vol. 1: Mechanisms of imitation and imitation in animals (pp. 101–18). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  51. Gallese, V. (2007). Before and below ‘theory of mind’: Embodied simulation and the neural correlates of social cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362, 659–669.Google Scholar
  52. Glenberg, A. M. (1997). What memory is for. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 1–19.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Glenberg, A. M., Witt, J. K., & Metcalfe, J. (2013). From the revolution to embodiment: 25 years of cognitive psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 573–585.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Godden, D. R., & Baddeley, A. D. (1975). Context-dependent memory in two natural environments: On land and underwater. British Journal of psychology, 66, 325–331.Google Scholar
  55. Goldinger, S. D., Papesh, M. H., Barnhart, A. S., Hansen, W. A., & Hout, M. C. (2016). The poverty of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 959–978.Google Scholar
  56. Halvorson, K. M., Bushinski, A., & Hilverman, C. (2019). The role of motor context in the beneficial effects of hand gesture on memory. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 1–11. Advance online publication.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-019-01734-3 Google Scholar
  57. Harnad, S. (1990). The symbol grounding problem. Physica D: Nonlinear Phenomena, 42, 335–346.Google Scholar
  58. Hauk, O., Johnsrude, I., & Pulvermüller, F. (2004). Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron, 41, 301–307.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Hegarty, M. (2004). Mechanical reasoning by mental simulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 280–285.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Hickok, G. (2014). The architecture of speech production and the role of the phoneme in speech processing. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29, 2–20.Google Scholar
  61. Hirschfeld, L. A., & Gelman, S. A. (Eds.). (1994). Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  62. Holm, L., & Mäntylä, T. (2007). Memory for scenes: Refixations reflect retrieval. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1664–1674.Google Scholar
  63. Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. W. (2008). Visible embodiment: Gestures as simulated action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 495–514.Google Scholar
  64. Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. W. (2010). Language, gesture, action! A test of the Gesture as Simulated Action framework. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 245–257.Google Scholar
  65. Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. W. (2019). Gesture as simulated action: Revisiting the framework. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26, 721–752.Google Scholar
  66. Hostetter, A. B., Alibali, M. W., & Shrager, S. M. (2011). If you don’t already know, I’m certainly not going to show you! Motivation to communicate affects gesture production. In G. Stam & M. Ishino (Eds.), Integrating gestures: The interdisciplinary nature of gesture (pp. 61–74). Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
  67. Ianì, F., & Bucciarelli, M. (2017). Mechanisms underlying the beneficial effect of a speaker’s gestures on the listener. Journal of Memory and Language, 96, 110–121.Google Scholar
  68. Ianì, F., & Bucciarelli, M. (2018). Relevance of the listener’s motor system in recalling phrases enacted by the speaker. Memory, 26, 1084–1092.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. Ianì, F., Cutica, I., & Bucciarelli, M. (2016). Timing of gestures: Gestures anticipating or simultaneous with speech as indexes of text comprehension in children and adults. Cognitive Science, 41, 1549–1566.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  70. Ianì, F., Foiadelli, A., & Bucciarelli, M. (2019). Mnemonic effects of action simulation from pictures and phrases. Acta Psychologica, 194, 37–50.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1998). Why people gesture when they speak. Nature, 396(6708), 228.  https://doi.org/10.1038/24300 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. James, W. (1950). The principles of psychology. Oxford, England: Dover Publications. (Original work published 1890)Google Scholar
  73. Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism for motor cognition. NeuroImage, 14, S103–S109.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Johansson, R., & Johansson, M. (2014). Look here, eye movements play a functional role in memory retrieval. Psychological Science, 25, 236–242.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. Kamermans, K. L., Pouw, W., Fassi, L., Aslanidou, A., Paas, F., & Hostetter, A. B. (2019). The role of gesture as simulated action in reinterpretation of mental imagery. Acta Psychologica, 197, 131–142.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  76. Kelly, S. D., Barr, D. J., Church, R. B., & Lynch, K. (1999). Offering a hand to pragmatic understanding: The role of speech and gesture in comprehension and memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 577–592.Google Scholar
  77. Kelly, S. D., Bryne, K., & Holler, J. (2011). Raising the ante of communication: Evidence for enhanced gesture use in high stakes situations. Information, 2, 579–593.Google Scholar
  78. Kelly, S. D., Manning, S. M., & Rodak, S. (2008). Gesture gives a hand to language and learning: Perspectives from cognitive neuroscience, developmental psychology and education. Language and Linguistics Compass, 2, 569–588.Google Scholar
  79. Kent, C., & Lamberts, K. (2008). The encoding–retrieval relationship: Retrieval as mental simulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 92–98.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  80. Keysers, C., & Gazzola, V. (2007). Integrating simulation and theory of mind: From self to social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 194–196.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  81. Khemlani, S. S., Mackiewicz, R., Bucciarelli, M., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2013). Kinematic mental simulations in abduction and deduction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 16766–16771.Google Scholar
  82. Kiefer, M., Sim, E. J., Herrnberger, B., Grothe, J., & Hoenig, K. (2008). The sound of concepts: Four markers for a link between auditory and conceptual brain systems. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 12224–12230.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  83. Kintsch, W. (2005). An overview of top-down and bottom-up effects in comprehension: The CI perspective. Discourse Processes, 39, 125–128.Google Scholar
  84. Kolers, P. A. (1973). Remembering operations. Memory & Cognition, 1, 347–355.Google Scholar
  85. Körner, A., Topolinski, S., & Strack, F. (2015). Routes to embodiment. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 940.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00940 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  86. Krauss, R. M. (1998). Why do we gesture when we speak?. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 54–60.Google Scholar
  87. Laeng, B., Bloem, I. M., D’Ascenzo, S., & Tommasi, L. (2014). Scrutinizing visual images: The role of gaze in mental imagery and memory. Cognition, 131, 263–283.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  88. Laeng, B., & Teodorescu, D. S. (2002). Eye scanpaths during visual imagery reenact those of perception of the same visual scene. Cognitive Science, 26, 207–231.Google Scholar
  89. Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to Western thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  90. Laird, J. D., Wagener, J. J., Halal, M., & Szegda, M. (1982). Remembering what you feel: Effects of emotion on memory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 646–657.Google Scholar
  91. LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
  92. Le Doux, J. (2002). Synaptic self: How our brains become who we are. New York, NY: Viking Penguin.Google Scholar
  93. Loftus, E. F., & Pickrell, J. E. (1995). The formation of false memories. Psychiatric Annals, 25, 720–725.Google Scholar
  94. Madan, C. R., & Singhal, A. (2012). Using actions to enhance memory: Effects of enactment, gestures, and exercise on human memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 507.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00507 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  95. Mahon, B. Z. (2015). The burden of embodied cognition. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 69, 172–178.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  96. Mahon, B. Z., & Caramazza, A. (2008). A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content. Journal of Physiology–Paris, 102, 59–70.Google Scholar
  97. Mahon, B. Z., & Hickok, G. (2016). Arguments about the nature of concepts: Symbols, embodiment, and beyond. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 941–958.Google Scholar
  98. Marotta, J. J., Behrmann, M., & Goodale, M. A. (1997). The removal of binocular cues disrupts the calibration of grasping in patients with visual form agnosia. Experimental Brain Research, 116, 113–121.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  99. Martin, A. (2007). The representation of object concepts in the brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 25–45.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  100. Masumoto, K., Yamaguchi, M., Sutani, K., Tsuneto, S., Fujita, A., & Tonoike, M. (2006). Reactivation of physical motor information in the memory of action events. Brain Research, 1101, 102–109.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  101. Mazzoni, G., Clark, A., & Nash, R. A. (2014). Disowned recollections: Denying true experiences undermines belief in occurrence but not judgments of remembering. Acta Psychologica, 145, 139–146.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  102. Mazzoni, G., Scoboria, A., & Harvey, L. (2010). Nonbelieved memories. Psychological Science, 21, 1334–1340.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  103. McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  104. Meteyard, L., Cuadrado, S. R., Bahrami, B., & Vigliocco, G. (2012). Coming of age: A review of embodiment and the neuroscience of semantics. Cortex, 48, 788–804.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  105. Michael, J., Sandberg, K., Skewes, J., Wolf, T., Blicher, J., Overgaard, M., & Frith, C. D. (2014). Continuous theta-burst stimulation demonstrates a causal role of premotor homunculus in action understanding. Psychological Science, 25, 963–972.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  106. Michalak, J., Mischnat, J., & Teismann, T. (2014). Sitting posture makes a difference—Embodiment effects on depressive memory bias. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 21, 519–524.Google Scholar
  107. Michalak, J., Rohde, K., & Troje, N. F. (2015). How we walk affects what we remember: Gait modifications through biofeedback change negative affective memory bias. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 46, 121–125.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  108. Morrel-Samuels, P., & Krauss, R. M. (1992). Word familiarity predicts temporal asynchrony of hand gestures and speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 615–622.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.18.3.615 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  109. Morse, A. F., Benitez, V. L., Belpaeme, T., Cangelosi, A., & Smith, L. B. (2015). Posture affects how robots and infants map words to objects. PLOS ONE, 10, e0116012.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0116012 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  110. Morsella, E., & Krauss, R. M. (2004). The role of gestures in spatial working memory and speech. American Journal of Psychology, 117, 411–424.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  111. Moskovitch, M. (2012). The contribution of research on autobiographical memory to past and present theories of memory consolidation. In D. Berntsen & D. Rubin (Eds.), Understanding autobiographical memory: Theories and approaches (pp. 91–113). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  112. Moulton, S. T., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2009). Imagining predictions: mental imagery as mental emulation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 1273–1280.Google Scholar
  113. Nathan, M. J., & Martinez, C. V. J. (2015). Gesture as model enactment: The role of gesture in mental model construction and inference making when learning from text. Learning: Research and Practice, 1, 4–37.Google Scholar
  114. Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L. W., Winkielman, P., Krauth-Gruber, S., & Ric, F. (2005). Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 184–211.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  115. Nilsson, L. G., Nyberg, L., Klingberg, T., Åberg, C., Persson, J., & Roland, P. E. (2000). Activity in motor areas while remembering action events. NeuroReport, 11, 2199–2201.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  116. Nyberg, L. (2002). Levels of processing: A view from functional brain imaging. Memory, 10, 345–348.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  117. Nyberg, L., Petersson, K. M., Nilsson, L. G., Sandblom, J., Åberg, C., & Ingvar, M. (2001). Reactivation of motor brain areas during explicit memory for actions. NeuroImage, 14, 521–528.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  118. Oosterwijk, S., Rotteveel, M., Fischer, A. H., & Hess, U. (2009). Embodied emotion concepts: How generating words about pride and disappointment influences posture. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 457–466.Google Scholar
  119. Otgaar, H., Howe, M. L., van Beers, J., van Hoof, R., Bronzwaer, N., & Smeets, T. (2015). The positive ramifications of false memories using a perceptual closure task. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 4, 43–50.Google Scholar
  120. Otgaar, H., Scoboria, A., & Mazzoni, G. (2014). On the existence and implications of nonbelieved memories. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23, 349–354.Google Scholar
  121. Otgaar, H., Scoboria, A., & Smeets, T. (2013). Experimentally evoking nonbelieved memories for childhood events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 717–730.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  122. Pecher, D. (2013). No role for motor affordances in visual working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 2–13.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  123. Pecher, D., de Klerk, R. M., Klever, L., Post, S., van Reenen, J. G., & Vonk, M. (2013). The role of affordances for working memory for objects. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 107–118.Google Scholar
  124. Persson, J., & Nyberg, L. (2000). Conjunction analysis of cortical activations common to encoding and retrieval. Microscopy Research and Technique, 51, 39–44.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  125. Pessoa, L. (2017). A network model of the emotional brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21, 357–371.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  126. Pezzulo, G., Barca, L., Bocconi, A. L., & Borghi, A. M. (2010). When affordances climb into your mind: Advantages of motor simulation in a memory task performed by novice and expert rock climbers. Brain and Cognition, 73, 68–73.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  127. Piaget, J. (1951). Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood (L. Gattegno & F. M. Hodsen, Trans.). New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
  128. Ping, R. M., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Beilock, S. L. (2014). Understanding gesture: Is the listener’s motor system involved?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 195–204.Google Scholar
  129. Poldrack, R. A., & Packard, M. G. (2003). Competition among multiple memory systems: Converging evidence from animal and human brain studies. Neuropsychologia, 41, 245–251.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  130. Prinz, W. (1990). A common coding approach to perception and action. In O. Neumann & W. Prinz (Eds.), Relationships between perception and action (pp. 167–201). Heidelberg, Germany: Springer.Google Scholar
  131. Pulvermüller, F. (2013). How neurons make meaning: Brain mechanisms for embodied and abstract-symbolic semantics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 458–470.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  132. Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1984). Computation and cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  133. Quak, M., Pecher, D., & Zeelenberg, R. (2014). Effects of motor congruence on visual working memory. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76, 2063–2070.Google Scholar
  134. Rimé, B. (1982). The elimination of visible behaviour from social interactions: Effects on verbal, nonverbal and interpersonal variables. European Journal of Social Psychology, 12, 113–129.Google Scholar
  135. Riskind, J. H. (1983). Nonverbal expressions and the accessibility of life experience memories: A congruence hypothesis. Social Cognition, 2, 62–86.Google Scholar
  136. Riskind, J. H., & Gotay, C. C. (1982). Physical posture: Could it have regulatory or feedback effects on motivation and emotion?. Motivation and Emotion, 6, 273–298.Google Scholar
  137. Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  138. Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., & Fogassi, L. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 131–141.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  139. Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. 28–49). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  140. Samson, R., & Brandon, R. (Eds.). (2007). Integrating evolution and development: From theory to practice. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  141. Scoboria, A., Boucher, C., & Mazzoni, G. (2015). Reasons for withdrawing belief in vivid autobiographical memories. Memory, 23, 545–562.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  142. Scoboria, A., Jackson, D. L., Talarico, J., Hanczakowski, M., Wysman, L., & Mazzoni, G. (2014). The role of belief in occurrence within autobiographical memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 1242–1258.Google Scholar
  143. Scoboria, A., Mazzoni, G., Kirsch, I., & Relyea, M. (2004). Plausibility and belief in autobiographical memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology: The Official Journal of the Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 18, 791-807.Google Scholar
  144. Seno, T., Kawabe, T., Ito, H., & Sunaga, S. (2013). Vection modulates emotional valence of autobiographical episodic memories. Cognition, 126, 115–120.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  145. Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  146. Shebani, Z., & Pulvermüller, F. (2013). Moving the hands and feet specifically impairs working memory for arm-and-leg-related action words. Cortex, 49, 222–231.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  147. Slotnick, S. D. (2004). Visual memory and visual perception recruit common neural substrates. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 3, 207–221.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  148. Solms, M., & Turnbull, O. (2002). The brain and the inner world: An introduction to the neuroscience of subjective experience. New York, NY: Karnac Books.Google Scholar
  149. Stepper, S., & Strack, F. (1993). Proprioceptive determinants of emotional and nonemotional feelings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 211–220.Google Scholar
  150. Stevanoni, E., & Salmon, K. (2005). Giving memory a hand: Instructing children to gesture enhances their event recall. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 29, 217–233.Google Scholar
  151. Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 768–777.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  152. Tettamanti, M., Buccino, G., Saccuman, M. C., Gallese, V., Danna, M., Scifo, P., … Perani, D. (2005). Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 273–281.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  153. Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2003). Neuroimaging studies of semantic memory: Inferring “how” from “where.” Neuropsychologia, 41, 280–292.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  154. Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (2004). Action priming by briefly presented objects. Acta Psychologica, 116, 185–203.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  155. Tulving, E. (1995). Organization of memory: Quo vadis? In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 839–853). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  156. Tulving, E., & Thomson, D. M. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80, 352–373.Google Scholar
  157. Ullman, M. T. (2004). Contributions of memory circuits to language: The declarative/procedural model. Cognition, 92, 231–270.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  158. Van Dam, W. O., Rueschemeyer, S. A., Bekkering, H., & Lindemann, O. (2013). Embodied grounding of memory: Toward the effects of motor execution on memory consolidation. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 2310–2328.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  159. Väljamäe, A., & Seno, T. (2016). Modulation of recognition memory for emotional images by vertical vection. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 39.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00039 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  160. Varela, F., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  161. Veenstra, L., Schneider, I. K., & Koole, S. L. (2017). Embodied mood regulation: The impact of body posture on mood recovery, negative thoughts, and mood-congruent recall. Cognition and Emotion, 31, 1361–1376.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  162. Versace, R., Vallet, G. T., Riou, B., Lesourd, M., Labeye, É., & Brunel, L. (2014). Act-In: An integrated view of memory mechanisms. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26, 280–306.Google Scholar
  163. Wagenmakers, E. J., Beek, T., Dijkhoff, L., Gronau, Q. F., Acosta, A., Adams , R. B., Jr, … Bulnes, L. C. (2016). Registered Replication Report: Strack, Martin, & Stepper (1988). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 917–928.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616674458 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  164. Wesp, R., Hesse, J., Keutmann, D., & Wheaton, K. (2001). Gestures maintain spatial imagery. The American Journal of Psychology, 114, 591–600.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  165. Wheeler, M. E., Petersen, S. E., & Buckner, R. L. (2000). Memory’s echo: Vivid remembering reactivates sensory-specific cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97, 11125–11129.Google Scholar
  166. Wilkes, C., Kydd, R., Sagar, M., & Broadbent, E. (2017). Upright posture improves affect and fatigue in people with depressive symptoms. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 54, 143–149.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  167. Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 625–636.Google Scholar
  168. Wilson, M., & Knoblich, G. (2005). The case for motor involvement in perceiving conspecifics. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 460–473.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  169. Yang, S. J., Gallo, D. A., & Beilock, S. L. (2009). Embodied memory judgments: A case of motor fluency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 1359–1365.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  170. Zatti, A., & Zarbo, C. (2015). Embodied and exbodied mind in clinical psychology: A proposal for a psycho-social interpretation of mental disorders. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 236.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Dipartimento di PsicologiaUniversità di TorinoTurinItaly

Personalised recommendations