Advertisement

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

, Volume 26, Issue 6, pp 1958–1966 | Cite as

A common mechanism underlying choice’s influence on preference and memory

  • Sarah DuBrow
  • Elizabeth A. Eberts
  • Vishnu P. MurtyEmail author
Brief Report

Abstract

Individual control over learning leads to better memory outcomes, yet it is still unclear which aspects of control matter. One’s sense of agency could be a key component, but it can be challenging to dissociate it from its consequences on the environment. Here we used a paradigm in which participants in one condition had the opportunity to choose between cues (choice condition) and in another were instructed which cue to select (fixed condition). Because the cues had no effect on the memoranda, we could isolate the effect of choice on memory. Participants also rated the cues for preference before and after encoding, allowing us to test how the number of times a cue was chosen affected its preference. By pooling multiple behavioral studies, we were able to use an individual differences approach to examine the relationship between choice effects on preference and memory. Replicating previous work, we found that immediate and delayed (24-h) recognition memory was higher for items encountered in the choice condition. We also found that cues that were selected more often increased their preference in the choice condition, but actually decreased their preference in the fixed condition, suggesting that choice engaged value-related processes. Critically, we found a positive across-subjects relationship between choice memory enhancements and choice-induced preference change for delayed but not for immediate memory. These data suggest that a shared value-based mechanism enhances preference for choice cues and memory consolidation of the choice outcomes. Thus, the value of choice may play an important role in learning enhancements.

Keywords

Choice Consolidation Decision making Episodic memory 

Notes

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by National Institutes of Health (NIH) grant R01 MH074682. V.P.M. is supported by NIH grant K01 MH111991. We also thank Lila Davachi for her support in experiment design and data collection.

Open practices statement

The data and materials for all experiments are available at https://osf.io/jmt4q/. None of the experiments were preregistered.

References

  1. Ariely, D., & Norton, M. I. (2008). How actions create—not just reveal—preferences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 13–16.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.10.008 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Chambon, V., Sidarus, N., & Haggard, P. (2014). From action intentions to action effects: how does the sense of agency come about? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 320.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00320 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  3. Fujiwara, J., Usui, N., Park, S. Q., Williams, T., Iijima, T., Taira, M., … Tobler, P. N. (2013). Value of freedom to choose encoded by the human brain. Journal of Neurophysiology, 110, 1915–1929.  https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01057.2012 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  4. Gruber, M. J., & Rangnath, C (2019). How curiosity enhances hippocampus-dependent memory. OSF preprints.  https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/5v6nm
  5. Gruber, M. J., Ritchey, M., Wang, S.-F., Doss, M. K., & Ranganath, C. (2016). Post-learning hippocampal dynamics promote preferential retention of rewarding events. Neuron, 89, 1110–1120.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2016.01.017 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  6. Gureckis, T. M., & Markant, D. B. (2012). Self-directed learning: A cognitive and computational perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 464–481.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612454304 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Haggard, P., & Tsakiris, M. (2009). The experience of agency: Feelings, judgments, and responsibility. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 242–246.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01644.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Kang, M. J., Hsu, M., Krajbich, I. M., Loewenstein, G., McClure, S. M., Wang, J. T., & Camerer, C. F. (2009). The wick in the candle of learning: epistemic curiosity activates reward circuitry and enhances memory. Psychological Science, 20, 963–973.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02402.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Kornell, N., & Metcalfe, J. (2006). Study efficacy and the region of proximal learning framework. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 609–622.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.609 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Kornell, N., & Son, L. K. (2009). Learners’ choices and beliefs about self-testing. Memory, 17, 493–501.  https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210902832915 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. LaBar, K. S., & Cabeza, R. (2006). Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 54–64.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1825 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Leotti, L. A., & Delgado, M. R. (2011). The inherent reward of choice. Psychological Science, 22, 1310–1318.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611417005 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  13. Leotti, L. A., & Delgado, M. R. (2014). The value of exercising control over monetary gains and losses. Psychological Science, 25, 596–604.  https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613514589 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  14. Leotti, L. A., Iyengar, S. S., & Ochsner, K. N. (2010). Born to choose: The origins and value of the need for control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 457–463.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.08.001 CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  15. Markant, D., DuBrow, S., Davachi, L., & Gureckis, T. M. (2014). Deconstructing the effect of self-directed study on episodic memory. Memory & Cognition, 42, 1211–1224.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0435-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Marvin, C. B., & Shohamy, D. (2016). Curiosity and reward: Valence predicts choice and information prediction errors enhance learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145, 266–272.  https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000140 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Metcalfe, J., & Greene, M. J. (2007). Metacognition of agency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 184–199.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.136.2.184 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Miendlarzewska, E. A., Bavelier, D., & Schwartz, S. (2016). Influence of reward motivation on human declarative memory. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 61, 156–176.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.11.015 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Murayama, K., & Kitagami, S. (2014). Consolidation power of extrinsic rewards: Reward cues enhance long-term memory for irrelevant past events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 15–20.  https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031992 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Murayama, K., & Kuhbandner, C. (2011). Money enhances memory consolidation—but only for boring material. Cognition, 119, 120–124.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.01.001 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Murty, V. P., & Adcock, R. A. (2017). Distinct medial temporal lobe network states as neural contexts for motivated memory formation. In D. E. Hannula & M. C. Duff (Eds.), The hippocampus from cells to systems: Structure, connectivity, and functional contributions to memory and flexible cognition (pp. 467–501). Cham, Switzerland: Springer.  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-50406-3_15 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Murty, V. P., DuBrow, S., & Davachi, L. (2015). The simple act of choosing influences declarative memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 35, 6255–6264.  https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4181-14.2015 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Murty, V. P., DuBrow, S., & Davachi, L. (2019). Decision-making increases episodic memory via postencoding consolidation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 31, 1308–1317.  https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01321 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Nairne, J. S., Pandeirada, J. N. S., & Thompson, S. R. (2008). Adaptive memory: The comparative value of survival processing. Psychological Science, 19, 176–180.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02064.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Nairne, J. S., Thompson, S. R., & Pandeirada, J. N. S. (2007). Adaptive memory: Survival processing enhances retention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 263–273.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.33.2.263 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Patall, E. A., Cooper, H., & Robinson, J. C. (2008). The effects of choice on intrinsic motivation and related outcomes: A meta-analysis of research findings. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 270–300.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.270 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Patil, A., Murty, V. P., Dunsmoor, J. E., Phelps, E. A., & Davachi, L. (2017). Reward retroactively enhances memory consolidation for related items. Learning and Memory, 24, 65–69.  https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.042978.116 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Pearce, J. M., & Hall, G. (1980). A model for Pavlovian learning: Variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychological Review, 87, 532–552.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.87.6.532 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Rotem-Turchinski, N., Ramaty, A., & Mendelsohn, A. (2019). The opportunity to choose enhances long-term episodic memory. Memory, 27, 431–440.  https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2018.1515317 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Rouhani, N., Norman, K. A., & Niv, Y. (2018). Dissociable effects of surprising reward on learning and memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44, 1430–1443.  https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000518 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Steiger, J. H. (1980). Tests for comparing elements of a correlation matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 87, 245–251.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.87.2.245 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Voss, J. L., Gonsalves, B. D., Federmeier, K. D., Tranel, D., & Cohen, N. J. (2011a). Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitional exploratory behavior enhances learning. Nature Neuroscience, 14, 115–120.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2693 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Voss, J. L., Warren, D. E., Gonsalves, B. D., Federmeier, K. D., Tranel, D., & Cohen, N. J. (2011b). Spontaneous revisitation during visual exploration as a link among strategic behavior, learning, and the hippocampus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, E402–E409.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1100225108 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Wenke, D., Fleming, S. M., & Haggard, P. (2010). Subliminal priming of actions influences sense of control over effects of action. Cognition, 115, 26–38.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.10.016 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Wimmer, G. E., Braun, E. K., Daw, N. D., & Shohamy, D. (2014). Episodic memory encoding interferes with reward learning and decreases striatal prediction errors. Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 14901–14912.  https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0204-14.2014 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • Sarah DuBrow
    • 1
  • Elizabeth A. Eberts
    • 2
  • Vishnu P. Murty
    • 2
    Email author
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of OregonEugeneUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyTemple UniversityPhiladelphiaUSA

Personalised recommendations