Advertisement

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

, Volume 26, Issue 4, pp 1310–1316 | Cite as

The mnemonic effect of choice

  • Michelle E. CoverdaleEmail author
  • James S. Nairne
Article

Abstract

Making choices during encoding leads to superior memory compared with having the same choices made for you. Evidence also suggests that chosen items might be more memorable than unchosen alternatives. In prior experiments, an incidental memory advantage was found for chosen over unchosen items when participants chose which one of two words would be more useful to a situation. However, it remains uncertain whether this mnemonic benefit is due to the act of choosing or to a better “fit” of chosen items to the encoding situation (congruity). In the present research, we conducted two experiments to dissociate choice and congruity effects. In both experiments, we manipulated choice and congruity and showed mnemonic benefits for chosen words over unchosen words and for congruent words over incongruent words, but these effects did not interact. There is apparently a unique mnemonic benefit for chosen words that cannot be explained by their “fit” to the encoding task.

Notes

Acknowledgements

This research was supported, in part, by a grant from the National Science Foundation (BCS-1532345). The authors wish to thank Josefa Pandeirada for comments regarding this research.

References

  1. Beggan, J. K. (1992). On the social nature of nonsocial perception: The mere ownership effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62(2), 229–237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Castel, A. D., Farb, N. A., & Craik, F. I. (2007). Memory for general and specific value information in younger and older adults: Measuring the limits of strategic control. Memory & Cognition, 35(4), 689–700.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Coltheart, M. (1981). The MRC psycholinguistic database. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33, 497–505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Coverdale, M. E., Pandeirada, J. N. S., & Nairne, J. S. (in press). Survival processing in a novel choice procedure. American Journal of Psychology.Google Scholar
  5. Craik, F. I., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104(3), 268–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Cunningham, S. J., Brady–van den Bos, M., & Turk, D. J. (2011). Exploring the effects of ownership and choice on self-memory biases. Memory, 19(5), 449–461.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 1149–1160. Google Scholar
  8. Festinger, L., & Carlsmith, J. (1959). Cognitive consequences of forced compliance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58(2), 203–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Fisher, R. P., & Craik, F. I. M. (1977). Interaction between encoding and retrieval operations in cued recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 3, 701–711.Google Scholar
  10. Izuma, K., Matsumoto, M., Murayama, K., Samejima, K., Sadato, N., & Matsumoto, K. (2010). Neural correlates of cognitive dissonance and choice-induced preference change. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the Unites States of America, 107(51), 22014–22019.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1011879108 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Markant, D., DuBrow, S., Davachi, L., & Gureckis, T. M. (2014). Deconstructing the effect of self-directed study on episodic memory. Memory & Cognition, 42(8), 1211–1224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Masson, M. E. (2011). A tutorial on a practical Bayesian alternative to null-hypothesis significance testing. Behavior research methods, 43(3), 679–690.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Murty, V. P., DuBrow, S., & Davachi, L. (2015). The simple act of choosing influences declarative memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(16), 6255–6264.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Murty, V. P., DuBrow, S., & Davachi, L. (2018). Decision-making increases episodic memory via postencoding consolidation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. Advanced online publication.  https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01321
  15. Nairne, J. S., Thompson, S. R., & Pandeirada, J. N. S. (2007). Adaptive memory: Survival processing enhances retention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33(2), 263–273.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Perlmuter, L. C., Monty, R. A., & Kimble, G. A. (1971). Effect of choice on paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 91(1), 47–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Schulman, A. I. (1974). Memory for words recently classified. Memory & Cognition, 2(1), 47–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Stefanidi, A., Ellis, D. M., & Brewer, G. A. (2018). Free recall dynamics in value-directed remembering. Journal of Memory and Language, 100, 18–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Van Overschelde, J. P., Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2004). Category norms: An updated and expanded version of the Battig and Montague (1969) norms. Journal of Memory and Language, 50(3), 289–335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Voss, J. L., Gonsalves, B. D., Federmeier, K. D., Tranel, D., & Cohen, N. J. (2011). Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitional exploratory behavior enhances learning. Nature Neuroscience, 14(1), 115–120.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Watanabe, T., & Soraci, S. A. (2004). The self-choice effect from a multiple-cue perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(1), 168–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Psychological SciencesPurdue UniversityWest LafayetteUSA

Personalised recommendations