NEVER forget: negative emotional valence enhances recapitulation

  • Holly J. Bowen
  • Sarah M. Kark
  • Elizabeth A. Kensinger
Theoretical Review

Abstract

A hallmark feature of episodic memory is that of “mental time travel,” whereby an individual feels they have returned to a prior moment in time. Cognitive and behavioral neuroscience methods have revealed a neurobiological counterpart: Successful retrieval often is associated with reactivation of a prior brain state. We review the emerging literature on memory reactivation and recapitulation, and we describe evidence for the effects of emotion on these processes. Based on this review, we propose a new model: Negative Emotional Valence Enhances Recapitulation (NEVER). This model diverges from existing models of emotional memory in three key ways. First, it underscores the effects of emotion during retrieval. Second, it stresses the importance of sensory processing to emotional memory. Third, it emphasizes how emotional valence – whether an event is negative or positive – affects the way that information is remembered. The model specifically proposes that, as compared to positive events, negative events both trigger increased encoding of sensory detail and elicit a closer resemblance between the sensory encoding signature and the sensory retrieval signature. The model also proposes that negative valence enhances the reactivation and storage of sensory details over offline periods, leading to a greater divergence between the sensory recapitulation of negative and positive memories over time. Importantly, the model proposes that these valence-based differences occur even when events are equated for arousal, thus rendering an exclusively arousal-based theory of emotional memory insufficient. We conclude by discussing implications of the model and suggesting directions for future research to test the tenets of the model.

Keywords

Emotion Encoding Reactivation Retrieval Memory 

Notes

Author note

All authors contributed to the literature review and development of the model and are listed in alphabetical order.

Manuscript preparation was supported by grant R01MH080833 from the National Institutes of Health (to EAK) and by a National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship DGE1258923 (to SMK). We thank all the attendees of the Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience laboratory meetings for their helpful discussion and critique of the ideas expressed in this manuscript.

Supplementary material

13423_2017_1313_MOESM1_ESM.docx (18 kb)
ESM 1 (DOCX 17.7 kb)
13423_2017_1313_MOESM2_ESM.docx (56 kb)
ESM 2 (DOCX 45.2 kb)
13423_2017_1313_MOESM3_ESM.docx (28 kb)
ESM 3 (DOCX 16.7 kb)

References

  1. Abercrombie, H. C., Chambers, A. S., Greischar, L., & Monticelli, R. M. (2008). Orienting, emotion, and memory: Phasic and tonic variation in heart rate predicts memory for emotional pictures in men. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 90(4), 644–650.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., & Buchanan, T. W. (2005). Amygdala damage impairs emotional memory for gist but not details of complex stimuli. Nature Neuroscience, 8(4), 512–8.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Ahn, H. M., Kim, S. A., Hwang, I. J., Jeong, J. W., Kim, H. T., Hamann, S., & Kim, S. H. (2015). The effect of cognitive reappraisal on long‐term emotional experience and emotional memory. Journal of Neuropsychology, 9(1), 64–76. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12035
  4. Anderson, A. K., Wais, P. E., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2006). Emotion enhances remembrance of neutral events past. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(5), 1599–1604.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Chapter: Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In K. W. Spence & J. T. Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Volume 2) (pp. 89–195). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  6. Aupée, A. M. (2007). A detrimental effect of emotion on picture recollection. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 48(1), 7–11.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Barber, S. J., Opitz, P. C., Martins, B., Sakaki, M., & Mather, M. (2016). Thinking about a limited future enhances the positivity of younger and older adults' recall: Support for socioemotional selectivity theory. Memory & Cognition, 44(6), 869–82. doi: 10.3758/s13421-016-0612-0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Batterink, L. J., Creery, J. D., & Paller, K. A. (2016). Phase of spontaneous slow oscillations during sleep influences memory-related processing of auditory cues. Journal of Neuroscience, 36(4), 1401–1409. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3175-15.2016 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 323–370. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.5.4.323 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bayer, J., Schultz, H., Gamer, M., & Sommer, T. (2014). Menstrual-cycle dependent fluctuations in ovarian hormones affect emotional memory. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 110, 55–63.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bendor, D., & Wilson, M. A. (2012). Biasing the content of hippocampal replay during sleep. Nature Neuroscience, 15(10), 1439–1444. doi: 10.1038/nn.3203 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Bennion, K. A., Ford, J. H., Murray, B. D., & Kensinger, E. A. (2013). Oversimplification in the study of emotional memory. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society, 19(9), 953–61. doi: 10.1017/S1355617713000945 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Bennion, K.A., Payne, J.D., & Kensinger, E.A. (2017). Residual effects of emotion are reflected in enhanced visual activity after sleep. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience. Google Scholar
  14. Beyeler, A., Namburi, P., Glober, G.F., Simonnet, C., Calhoon, G.G., … & Tye, K.M. (2016). Divergent routing of positive and negative information from the amygdala during memory retrieval. Neuron, 90(2), 348–61. doi:  10.1016/j.neuron.2016.03.004
  15. Bless, H., & Schwartz, N. (1999). Sufficient and necessary condition in dual process models: The case of mood and information processing. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual process theories in social psychology (pp. 423–440). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  16. Bos, M. G., Schuijer, J., Lodestijn, F., Beckers, T., & Kindt, M. (2014). Stress enhances reconsolidation of declarative memory. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 46, 102–13. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.04.011 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Bosch, S. E., Jehee, J. F., Fernández, G., & Doeller, C. F. (2014). Reinstatement of associative memories in early visual cortex is signaled by the hippocampus. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(22), 7493–7500. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0805-14.2014 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Bowen, H. J., & Kark, S. M. (2016). Commentary: Episodic memory retrieval functionally relies on very rapid reactivation of sensory information. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 196. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00196. eCollection 2016.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Bowen, H.J. & Kensinger, E.A. (2016). Recapitulation of Emotional Source Context during Memory Retrieval. Cortex, in press. Google Scholar
  20. Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129–148.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Bower, G. H., & Forgas, J. P. (2000). Affect, memory, and social cognition. In E. Eich, J. F. Kihlstrom, G. H. Bower, J. P. Forgas, & P. M. Niedenthal (Eds.), Cognition and emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  22. Bradley, M. M., Greenwald, M. K., Petry, M. C., & Lang, P. J. (1992). Remembering pictures: Pleasure and arousal in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(2), 379–390.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Brewin, C. R., Gregory, J. D., Lipton, M., & Burgess, N. (2010). Intrusive images in psychological disorders: Characteristics, neural mechanisms, and treatment implications. Psychology Review, 117(1), 210–232. doi: 10.1037/a0018113 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Brewin, C. R., Kleiner, J. S., Vasterling, J. J., & Field, A. P. (2007). Memory for emotionally neutral information in posttraumatic stress disorder: A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(3), 448–463. doi: 10.1037/0021-843x.116.3.448 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Bridge, D. J., & Voss, J. L. (2014). Active retrieval facilitates across-episode binding by modulating the content of memory. Neuropsychologia, 63, 154–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.024 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Brown, R., & Kulik, J. (1977). Flashbulb memories. Cognition, 5(1), 73–99. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(77)90018-X CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Buchanan, T. W. (2007). Retrieval of emotional memories. Psychological Bulletin, 133(5), 761–79.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Buchanan, T. W., Tranel, D., & Adolphs, R. (2005). Emotional autobiographical memories in amnesic patients with medial temporal lobe damage. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(12), 3151–60.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Buchanan, T. W., Tranel, D., & Adolphs, R. (2006). Memories for emotional autobiographical events following unilateral damage to medial temporal lobe. Brain, 129(Pt 1), 115–27.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Buckner, R. L., & Wheeler, M. E. (2001). The cognitive neuroscience of remembering. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 2(9), 624–634. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2010.503602 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Buhry, L., Azizi, A. H., & Cheng, S. (2011). Reactivation, replay, and preplay: How it might all fit together. Neural Plasticity, 2011, 203462. doi: 10.1155/2011/203462 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Buzsaki, G. (1998). Memory consolidation during sleep: A neurophysiological perspective. Journal of Sleep Research, 7(Suppl 1), 17–23.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Cahill, L., & Alkire, M. T. (2003). Epinephrine enhancement of human memory consolidation: Interaction with arousal at encoding. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 79(2), 194–198.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Cahill, L., & McGaugh, J. L. (1998). Mechanisms of emotional arousal and lasting declarative memory. Trends in Neurosciences, 21(7), 294–9.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Calvo, M. G., & Lang, P. J. (2004). Gaze patterns when looking at emotional pictures: Motivationally biased attention. Motivation and Emotion, 28(3), 221–243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Christianson, S. A. (1992). Emotional stress and eyewitness memory: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 112(2), 284–309.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Claire, M. D., Sophie, D., Claudia, P., Philippe, M., & Eliane, R. P. (2016). Verbal emotional memory in a case with left amygdala damage. Neurocase, 22(1), 45–54. doi: 10.1080/13554794.2015.1037843 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Costafreda, S. G., Brammer, M. J., David, A. S., & Fu, C. H. (2008). Predictors of amygdala activation during the processing of emotional stimuli: A meta-analysis of 385 PET and fMRI studies. Brain Research Reviews, 58(1), 57–70.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Danker, J. F., & Anderson, J. R. (2010). The ghosts of brain states past: Remember reactivates the brain regions engaged during encoding. Psychological Bulletin and Review, 136(1), 87–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. D'Argembeau, A., Comblain, C., & Van der Linden, M. (2005). Affective valence and the self-reference effect: Influence of retrieval conditions. British Journal of Psychology, 96(4), 457–66.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Daselaar, S. M., Rice, H. J., Greenberg, D. L., Cabeza, R., LaBar, K. S., & Rubin, D. C. (2008). The spatiotemporal dynamics of autobiographical memory: Neural correlates of recall, emotional intensity, and reliving. Cerebral Cortex, 18(1), 217–229. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm048 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Deuker, L., Olligs, J., Fell, J., Kranz, T. A., Mormann, F., Montag, C., … & Axmacher, N. (2013). Memory consolidation by replay of stimulus-specific neural activity. J Neurosci, 33(49), 19373–19383. doi:  10.1523/jneurosci.0414-13.2013
  43. Dewhurst, S. A., & Parry, L. A. (2000). Emotionality, distinctiveness, and recollective experience. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 12(4), 541–551. doi: 10.1080/095414400750050222 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Dolcos, F., & Cabeza, R. (2002). Event-related potentials of emotional memory: Encoding pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral pictures. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2(3), 252–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Dolcos, F., LaBar, K. S., & Cabeza, R. (2004). Interaction between the amygdala and the medial temporal lobe memory system predicts better memory for emotional events. Neuron, 42(5), 855–863.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Donderi, D. C. (2006). Visual complexity: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 73–97. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.73 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Dougal, S., & Rotello, C. M. (2007). "Remembering" emotional words is based on response bias, not recollection. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14(3), 423–9.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Dunsmoor, J. E., Murty, V. P., Davachi, L., & Phelps, E. A. (2015). Emotional learning selectively and retroactively strengthens memories for related events. Nature, 520(7547), 345–348. doi: 10.1038/nature14106 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66(3), 183–201. doi: 10.1037/h0047707 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Ego-Stengel, V., & Wilson, M. A. (2010). Disruption of ripple-associated hippocampal activity during rest impairs spatial learning in the rat. Hippocampus, 20(1), 1–10. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20707 PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  51. Eich, E. (1995). Mood as a mediator of place dependent memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124(3), 293–308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Emery, L., & Hess, T. M. (2008). Viewing instructions impact emotional memory differently in older and young adults. Psychology and Aging, 23(1), 2–12.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Fastenrath, M., Coynel, D., Spalek, K., Milnik, A., Gschwind, L., Roozendaal, B., .… & de Quervain, D. J. (2014). Dynamic modulation of amygdala-hippocampal connectivity by emotional arousal. J Neurosci, 34(42), 13935–13947. doi:  10.1523/jneurosci.0786-14.2014
  54. Fenker, D. B., Schott, B. H., Richardson-Klavehn, A., Heinze, H. J., & Düzel, E. (2005). Recapitulating emotional context: Activity of amygdala, hippocampus and fusiform cortex during recollection and familiarity. European Journal of Neuroscience, 21(7), 1993–9.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Fernández-Rey, J., & Redondo, J. (2007). Recognition memory for pictorial stimuli: Biasing effects of stimulus emotionality. Psicothema, 19(3), 375–80.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Ferré, P., Fraga, I., Comesaña, M., & Sánchez-Casas, R. (2015). Memory for emotional words: The role of semantic relatedness, encoding task and affective valence. Cognition and Emotion, 29(8), 1401–1410.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Ford, J. H., & Kensinger, E. A. (2016). Effects of internal and external vividness on hippocampal connectivity during memory retrieval. Neurobiology of Aging, 134, 78–90.Google Scholar
  58. Ford, J. H., Morris, J. A., & Kensinger, E. A. (2014). Effects of emotion and emotional valence on the neural correlates of episodic memory search and elaboration. Journal Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(4), 825–839. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00529 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Foster, D. J., & Wilson, M. A. (2006). Reverse replay of behavioural sequences in hippocampal place cells during the awake state. Nature, 440(7084), 680–683. doi: 10.1038/nature04587 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Gavazzeni, J., Andersson, T., Backman, L., Wiens, S., & Fischer, H. (2012). Age, gender, and arousal in recognition of negative and neutral pictures 1 year later. Psychology and Aging, 27(4), 1039–1052. doi: 10.1037/a0027946 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Glaser, E., Mendrek, A., Germain, M., Lakis, N., & Lavoie, M. E. (2012). Sex differences in memory of emotional images: A behavioral and electrophysiological investigation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 85(1), 17–26. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.01.007 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Goerke, M., Muller, N. G., & Cohrs, S. (2017). Sleep-dependent memory consolidation and its implications for psychiatry. J Neural Transm (Vienna), 124(Suppl 1), 163–178. doi: 10.1007/s00702-015-1476-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Gottfried, J. A., Smith, A. P. R., Rugg, M. D., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Remembrance of odors past: Human olfactory cortex in cross-modal recognition memory. Neuron, 42(4), 687–695. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00270-3 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Haas, B. W., & Canli, T. (2008). Emotional memory function, personality structure and psychopathology: A neural system approach to the identification of vulnerability markers. Brain Research Reviews, 58(1), 71–84. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.10.014 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Haggerty, D. C., & Ji, D. (2014). Initiation of sleep-dependent cortical-hippocampal correlations at wakefulness-sleep transition. Journal of Neurophysiology, 112(7), 1763–1774. doi: 10.1152/jn.00783.2013 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Hamann, S. (2001). Cognitive and neural mechanisms of emotional memory. Trends in Cognitive Science, 5(9), 394–400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Hamann, S. B., Ely, T. D., Grafton, S. T., & Kilts, C. D. (1999). Amygdala activity related to enhanced memory for pleasant and aversive stimuli. Nature Neuroscience, 2(3), 289–93.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Hamann, S., & Mao, H. (2002). Positive and negative emotional verbal stimuli elicit activity in the left amygdala. Neuroreport, 13(1), 15–9.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Harrington, M. O., Pennington, K., & Durrant, S. J. (2017). The 'affect tagging and consolidation' (ATaC) model of depression vulnerability. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 140, 43–51. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2017.02.003 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Hermans, E. J., Battaglia, F. P., Atsak, P., de Voogd, L. D., Fernandez, G., & Roozendaal, B. (2014). How the amygdala affects emotional memory by altering brain network properties. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 112, 2–16. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.02.005 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Holland, A. C., & Kensinger, E. A. (2010). Emotion and autobiographical memory. Physics of Life Reviews, 7(1), 88–131. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2010.01.006 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Holland, A. C., & Kensinger, E. A. (2012). Younger, middle-aged, and older adults' memories for the 2008 U.S. Presidential Election. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 1(3), 163–170.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Ji, D., & Wilson, M. A. (2007). Coordinated memory replay in the visual cortex and hippocampus during sleep. Nature Neuroscience, 10(1), 100–107. doi: 10.1038/nn1825 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Johnson, J. D., McDuff, S. G., Rugg, M. D., & Norman, K. A. (2009). Recollection, familiarity, and cortical reinstatement: A multivoxel pattern analysis. Neuron, 63(5), 697–708.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Johnson, J. D., Minton, B. R., & Rugg, M. D. (2008). Content dependence of the electrophysiological correlates of recollection. NeuroImage, 39(1), 406–416. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.08.050 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Johnson, J. D., Price, M. H., & Leiker, E. K. (2015). Episodic retrieval involves early and sustained effects of reactivating information from encoding. NeuroImage, 106, 300–310. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.013 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Kamp, S. M., Potts, G. F., & Donchin, E. (2015). On the roles of distinctiveness and semantic expectancies in episodic encoding of emotional words. Psychophysiology, 52(12), 1599–1609.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Kark, S. M., & Kensinger, E. A. (2015a). Effect of emotional valence on retrieval-related recapitulation of encoding activity in the ventral visual stream. Neuropsychologia, 78, 221–230. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.10.014 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Kark, S. M., & Kensinger, E. A. (2015b). Increases in arousal influence encoding processes in valence-specific ways. Chicago: Poster session presented at the Society for Neuroscience Annual Meeting.Google Scholar
  80. Keightley, M. L., Chiew, K. S., Anderson, J. A., & Grady, C. L. (2011). Neural correlates of recognition memory for emotional faces and scenes. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6(1), 24–37. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq003 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Kensinger, E. A. (2015). The future can shape memory for the present. Trends in Cognitive Science, 19(4), 179–180. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.02.008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Kensinger, E. A., & Choi, E. S. (2009). When side matters: Hemispheric processing and the visual specificity of emotional memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(1), 247–53. doi: 10.1037/a0013414 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  83. Kensinger, E. A., & Corkin, S. (2004). Two routes to emotional memory: Distinct neural processes for valence and arousal. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101(9), 3310–3315. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0306408101 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Kensinger, E. A., Garoff-Eaton, R. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2007a). How negative emotion enhances the visual specificity of a memory. Journal Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(11), 1872–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Kensinger, E. A., Garoff-Eaton, R. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2007b). Effects of emotion on memory specificity in young and older adults. Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 62(4), 208–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Kensinger, E. A., O'Brien, J. L., Swanberg, K., Garoff-Eaton, R. J., & Schacter, D. L. (2007). The effects of emotional content on reality-monitoring performance in young and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 22(4), 752–64. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.22.4.752 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2006a). Amygdala activity is associated with the successful encoding of item, but not source, information for positive and negative stimuli. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(9), 2564–2570.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2006b). When the Red Sox shocked the Yankees: Comparing negative and positive memories. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13(5), 757–63.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2007). Remembering the specific visual details of presented objects: Neuroimaging evidence for effects of emotion. Neuropsychologia, 45(13), 2951–2962. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.05.024 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2008a). Memory and emotion. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of Emotions. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  91. Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2008b). Neural processes supporting young and older adults’ emotional memories. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(7), 1161–1173.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Koenig, S., & Mecklinger, A. (2008). Electrophysiological correlates of encoding and retrieving emotional events. Emotion, 8(2), 162–73. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.2.162 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Kuhl, B. A., & Chun, M. M. (2014). Successful remembering elicits event-specific activity patterns in lateral parietal cortex. J Neurosci, 34(23), 8051–8060. doi:  10.1523/jneurosci.4328-13.2014 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Kuhl, B. A., Rissman, J., Chun, M. M., & Wagner, A. D. (2011). Fidelity of neural reactivation reveals competition between memories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 108(4).Google Scholar
  95. Kuhl, B. A., Rissman, J., & Wagner, A. D. (2012). Multi-voxel patterns of visual category representation during episodic encoding are predictive of subsequent memory. Neuropsychologia, 50(4), 458–469. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.09.002 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. LaBar, K. S., & Cabeza, R. (2006). Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(1), 54–64. doi: 10.1038/nrn1825 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. LaBar, K. S., Mesulam, M., Gitelman, D. R., & Weintraub, S. (2000). Emotional curiosity: Modulation of visuospatial attention by arousal is preserved in aging and early-stage Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychologia, 38(13), 1734–1740.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. LaBar, K. S., & Phelps, E. A. (1998). Arousal-mediated memory consolidation: Role of the medial temporal lobe in humans. Psychological Science, 9, 490–493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Lane, R.D., Ryan, L., Nadel, L., & Greenberg, L. (2015). Memory reconsolidation, emotional arousal, and the process of change in psychotherapy: New insights from brain science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 38. doi:  10.1017/S0140525X14000041
  100. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual (Technical Report A-8). Gainesville: University of Florida.Google Scholar
  101. Lavoie, M. E., & O’Connor, K. P. (2013). Effect of emotional valence on episodic memory stages as indexed by event-related potentials. World Journal of Neuroscience, 2013(3), 250–262.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Lehmann, M., Schreiner, T., Seifritz, E., & Rasch, B. (2016). Emotional arousal modulates oscillatory correlates of targeted memory reactivation during NREM, but not REM sleep. Science Reports, 6, 39229. doi: 10.1038/srep39229 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Leiker, E. K., & Johnson, J. D. (2014). Neural reinstatement and the amount of information recollected. Brain Research, 1582(September), 125–138. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.07.026 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Leiker, E. K., & Johnson, J. D. (2015). Pattern reactivation co-varies with activity in the core recollection network during source memory. Neuropsychologia, 75(May), 88–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.05.021 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. Lin, M., & Tanno, Y. (2012). Enhancing effects of post-learning stress on memory. Psychology, 3, 419–423. doi: 10.4236/psych.2012.35059 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  106. Liu, D. L., Graham, S., & Zorawski, M. (2008). Enhanced selective memory consolidation following post-learning pleasant and aversive arousal. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 89(1), 36–46.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Liu, W. H., Wang, L. Z., Zhao, S. H., Ning, Y. P., & Chan, R. C. (2012). Anhedonia and emotional word memory in patients with depression. Psychiatry Research, 200(2–3), 361–7. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.07.025 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  108. Loftus, E. F., Loftus, G. R., & Messo, J. (1987). Some facts about “weapon focus”. Law and Human Behavior, 11(1), 55–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  109. Louie, K., & Wilson, M. A. (2001). Temporally structured replay of awake hippocampal ensemble activity during rapid eye movement sleep. Neuron, 29(1), 145–156.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  110. MacKenzie, G., Powell, T. F., & Donaldson, D. I. (2015). Positive emotion can protect against source memory impairment. Cognition and Emotion, 29(2), 236–250.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  111. Maddock, R. J., & Frein, S. T. (2009). Reduced memory for the spatial and temporal context of unpleasant words. Cognition and Emotion, 23(1), 96–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  112. Maratos, E. J., & Rugg, M. D. (2001). Electrophysiological correlates of the retrieval of emotional and non-emotional context. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(7), 877–891.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  113. Markowitsch, H. J., Vandekerckhove, M. M., Lanfermann, H., & Russ, M. O. (2003). Engagement of lateral and medial prefrontal areas in the ecphory of sad and happy autobiographical memories. Cortex, 39(4–5), 643–65.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  114. Marshall, L., & Born, J. (2007). The contribution of sleep to hippocampus-dependent memory consolidation. Trends in Cognitive Science, 11(10), 442–450. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  115. Mather, M., Clewett, D., Sakaki, M., & Harley, C.W. (2016). Norepinephrine ignites local hot spots of neuronal excitation: How arousal amplifies selectivity in perception and memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, 1–75. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X15000667
  116. Mather, M., & Nesmith, K. (2008). Arousal-enhanced location memory for pictures. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(2), 449–464. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.01.004 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  117. Mather, M., & Sutherland, M. R. (2011). Arousal-biased competition in perception and memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(2), 114–133. doi: 10.1177/1745691611400234 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  118. McClelland, J. L., McNaughton, B. L., & O'Reilly, R. C. (1995). Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychology Review, 102(3), 419–457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  119. McGaugh, J. L. (2000). Memory--a century of consolidation. Science, 287(5451), 248–251.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  120. McGaugh, J. L. (2004). The amygdala modulates the consolidation of memories of emotionally arousing experiences. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 1–28. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144157 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  121. Mickley Steinmetz, K. R., Addis, D. R., & Kensinger, E. A. (2010). The effect of arousal on the emotional memory network depends on valence. NeuroImage, 53(1), 318–24. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.015 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  122. Mickley Steinmetz, K. R., & Kensinger, E. A. (2009). The effects of valence and arousal on the neural activity leading to subsequent memory. Psychophysiology, 46(6), 1190–1199.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  123. Mickley Steinmetz, K. R., Knight, A. G., & Kensinger, E. A. (2016). Neutral details associated with emotional events are encoded: Evidence from a cued recall paradigm. Cognition & Emotion, 30(7), 1352–60. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2015.1059317 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  124. Mickley Steinmetz, K. R., Schmidt, K., Zucker, H. R., & Kensinger, E. A. (2012). The effect of emotional arousal and retention delay on subsequent-memory effects. Cognitive Neuroscience, 3(3–4), 150–159. doi: 10.1080/17588928.2012.677421 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  125. Mickley, K., & Kensinger, E. A. (2008). Emotional valence influences the neural correlates associated with remembering and knowing. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 8(2), 143–152.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  126. Mitchell, K. J., Mather, M., Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., & Greene, E. J. (2006). A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of short-term source and item memory for negative pictures. Neuroreport, 17(14), 1543–1547. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000234743.50442.e5 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  127. Mneimne, M., Powers, A. S., Walton, K. E., Kosson, D. S., Fonda, S., & Simonetti, J. (2010). Emotional valence and arousal effects on memory and hemispheric asymmetries. Brain and Cognition, 74(1), 10–17.Google Scholar
  128. Morcom, A. M. (2014). Re-engaging with the past: Recapitulation of encoding operations during episodic retrieval. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 351.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  129. Morris, C. D., Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1977). Levels of processing versus transfer appropriate processing. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16(5), 519–533. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(77)80016-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  130. Murty, V. P., Ritchey, M., Adcock, R. A., & LaBar, K. S. (2011). Reprint of: fMRI studies of successful emotional memory encoding: A quantitative meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia, 49(4), 695–705. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.031 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  131. Nairne, J. S. (2002). The myth of the encoding-retrieval match. Memory, 10(5–6), 389–395. doi: 10.1080/09658210244000216 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  132. Neisser, U., & Harsch, N. (1992). Phantom flashbulbs: False recollections of hearing the news about Challenger. In E. Winograd & U. Neisser (Eds.), Affect and accuracy in recall (pp. 9–31). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511664069.003
  133. Norman, K. A., Polyn, S. M., Detre, G. J., & Haxby, J. V. (2006). Beyond mind-reading: Multi-voxel pattern analysis of fMRI data. Trends in Cognitive Science, 10(9), 424–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  134. Nummenmaa, L., Hyona, J., & Calvo, M. G. (2006). Eye movement assessment of selective attentional capture by emotional pictures. Emotion, 6(2), 257–268. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.2.257 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  135. Nummenmaa, L., Hyona, J., & Calvo, M. G. (2009). Emotional scene content drives the saccade generation system reflexively. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(2), 305–323. doi: 10.1037/a0013626 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  136. Nyberg, L., Habib, R., McIntosh, A. R., & Tulving, E. (2000). Reactivation of encoding-related brain activity during memory retrieval. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(20), 11120–11124. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.20.11120 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  137. Ochsner, K. N. (2000). Are affective events richly recollected or simply familiar? The experience and process of recognizing feelings past. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129(2), 242–261. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.129.2.242 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  138. Ohira, H., Winton, W. M., & Oyama, M. (1998). Effects of stimulus valence on recognition memory and endogenous eyeblinks: Further evidence for positive–negative asymmetry. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 986–993.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  139. Öhman, A. (1979). The orienting response, attention and learning: An information-processing perspective. In H. D. Kimmel, E. H. Van Olst, & J. F. Orlebeke (Eds.), The orienting reflex in human (pp. 55–80). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  140. O'Keefe, J. (1976). Place units in the hippocampus of the freely moving rat. Experimental Neurology, 51(1), 78–109.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  141. Oudiette, D., Antony, J. W., Creery, J. D., & Paller, K. A. (2013). The role of memory reactivation during wakefulness and sleep in determining which memories endure. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(15), 6672–6678. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.5497-12.2013 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  142. Oudiette, D., & Paller, K. A. (2013). Upgrading the sleeping brain with targeted memory reactivation. Trends in Cognitive Science, 17(3), 142–149. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.01.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  143. Paller, K. A., & Voss, J. L. (2004). Memory reactivation and consolidation during sleep. Learning and Memory, 11(6), 664–670. doi: 10.1101/lm.75704 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  144. Payne, J. D., Chambers, A. M., & Kensinger, E. A. (2012). Sleep promotes lasting changes in selective memory for emotional scenes. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 6, 108. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00108 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  145. Payne, J. D., & Kensinger, E. A. (2011). Sleep leads to changes in the emotional memory trace: Evidence from FMRI. Journal Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(6), 1285–97. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21526 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  146. Pessoa, L. (2005). To what extent are emotional visual stimuli processed without attention and awareness? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15(2), 188–96.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  147. Pessoa, L. (2010). Emotion and cognition and the amygdala: From "what is it?" to "what's to be done?". Neuropsychologia, 48(12), 3416–29. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.038 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  148. Phelps, E. A. (2004). Human emotion and memory: interactions of the amygdala and hippocampal complex. Curr Opin Neurobiol, 14(2), 198–202. doi:  10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.015 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  149. Phelps, E. A., & Sharot, T. (2008). How (and Why) emotion enhances the subjective sense of recollection. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(2), 147–152. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00565.x PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  150. Piefke, M., Weiss, P. H., Zilles, K., Markowitsch, H. J., & Fink, G. R. (2003). Differential remoteness and emotional tone modulate the neural correlates of autobiographical memory. Brain, 126(Pt 3), 650–68.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  151. Polyn, S. M., Natu, V. S., Cohen, J. D., Norman, K. A., & 5756. (2005). Category-specific cortical activity precedes retrieval during memory search. Science (New York, N.Y.), 310, 1963–6. doi: 10.1126/science.1117645 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  152. Pompili, A., Arnone, B., D'Amico, M., Federico, P., & Gasbarri, A. (2016). Evidence of estrogen modulation on memory processes for emotional content in healthy young women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 65, 94–101.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  153. Pourtois, G., Schettino, A., & Vuilleumier, P. (2013). Brain mechanisms for emotional influences on perception and attention: What is magic and what is not. Biological Psychology, 92(3), 492–512. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.02.007 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  154. Rasch, B., Spalek, K., Buholzer, S., Luechinger, R., Boesiger, P., Papassotiropoulos, A., & Quervain, D. F. (2007). Maintaining memories by reactivation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 17(6), 698-703. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.11.007
  155. Rasch, B., Büchel, C., Gais, S., & Born, J. (2007). Odor cues during slow-wave sleep prompt declarative memory consolidation. Science, 315(5817), 1426–9.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  156. Reichelt, A. C., & Lee, J. L. (2013). Memory reconsolidation in aversive and appetitive settings. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 118. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00118 PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  157. Reisberg, D., & Heuer, F. (2004). Memory for emotional events. In D. Reisberg & P. Hertel (Eds.), Memory and emotion (pp. 3–41). New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  158. Rimmele, U., Lackovic, S. F., Tobe, R. H., Leventhal, B. L., & Phelps, E. A. (2016). Beta-adrenergic blockade at memory encoding, but not retrieval, decreases the subjective sense of recollection. Journal Cognitive Neuroscience, 28(6), 895–907. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00941 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  159. Ritchey, M., Dolcos, F., & Cabeza, R. (2008). Role of amygdala connectivity in the persistence of emotional memories over time: An event-related fMRI investigation. Cerebral Cortex, 18(11), 2494–2504. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm262 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  160. Ritchey, M., LaBar, K. S., & Cabeza, R. (2011). Level of processing modulates the neural correlates of emotional memory formation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(4), 757–771.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  161. Ritchey, M., Wing, E. A., LaBar, K. S., & Cabeza, R. (2013). Neural Similarity Between Encoding and Retrieval is Related to Memory Via Hippocampal Interactions. Cerebral Cortex, 23(12), 2818–2828. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs258 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  162. Robinson-Riegler, G., & Wintonw, W. (1996). The Role of Conscious Recollection in Recognition of Affective Material: Evidence for Positive–negative Asymmetry. The Journal of General Psychology, 123(2), 93–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  163. Roozendaal, B., McEwen, B. S., & Chattarji, S. (2009). Stress, memory and the amygdala. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 423–433. doi: 10.1038/nrn2651 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  164. Rugg, M. D., Johnson, J. D., Park, H., & Uncapher, M. R. (2008a). Chapter 21 Encoding-retrieval overlap in human episodic memory: A functional neuroimaging perspective. In W. S. Sossin, J. C. Lacaille, V. F. Castellucci, & S. Belleville (Eds.), Essence of Memory. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  165. Rugg, M. D., Johnson, J. D., Park, H., & Uncapher, M. R. (2008b). Encoding-retrieval overlap in human episodic memory: A functional neuroimaging perspective. Progress in Brain Research, 169, 339–52. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(07)00021-0 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  166. Sava, A. A., Paquet, C., Dumurgier, J., Hugon, J., & Chainay, H. (2016). The role of attention in emotional memory enhancement in pathological and healthy aging. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 38(4), 434–54. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2015.1123225 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  167. Schmidt, L., Patnaik, P., & Kensinger, E. A. (2011). Emotion's influence on memory for spatial and temporal context. Cognition and Emotion, 25(2), 229–243.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  168. Schupp, H. T., Cuthbert, B. N., Bradley, M. M., Cacioppo, J. T., Ito, T., & Lang, P. J. (2000). Affective picture processing: The late positive potential is modulated by motivational relevance. Psychophysiology, 37(2), 257–61.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  169. Sharot, T., & Phelps, E. A. (2004). How arousal modulates memory: Disentangling the effects of attention and retention. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 4(3), 294–306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  170. Sharot, T., Verfaellie, M., & Yonelinas, A. P. (2007). How emotion strengthens the recollective experience: A time-dependent hippocampal process. PloS One, 2(10), e1068. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001068 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  171. Sharot, T., & Yonelinas, A. P. (2008). Differential time-dependent effects of emotion on recollective experience and memory for contextual information. Cognition, 106(1), 538–547. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.03.002 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  172. Simola, J., Torniainen, J., Moisala, M., Kivikangas, M., & Krause, C. M. (2013). Eye movement related brain responses to emotional scenes during free viewing. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 7, 41. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00041 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  173. Sirota, A., & Buzsaki, G. (2005). Interaction between neocortical and hippocampal networks via slow oscillations. Thalamus & Related Systems, 3(4), 245–259. doi: 10.1017/s1472928807000258 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  174. Slotnick, S. D. (2004). Visual memory and visual perception recruit common neural substrates. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 3(4), 207–221. doi: 10.1177/1534582304274070 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  175. Slotnick, S. D., & Thakral, P. P. (2011). Memory for motion and spatial location is mediated by contralateral and ipsilateral motion processing cortex. NeuroImage, 55(2), 794–800. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.077 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  176. Smith, A. P., Dolan, R. J., & Rugg, M. D. (2004). Event-related potential correlates of the retrieval of emotional and nonemotional context. Journal Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(5), 760–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  177. Smith, A. P., Henson, R. N., Dolan, R. J., & Rugg, M. D. (2004). fMRI correlates of the episodic retrieval of emotional contexts. NeuroImage, 22(2), 868–78.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  178. Smith, A. P., Henson, R. N., Rugg, M. D., & Dolan, R. J. (2005). Modulation of retrieval processing reflects accuracy of emotional source memory. Learning and Memory, 12(5), 472–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  179. Smith, A. P., Stephan, K. E., Rugg, M. D., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Task and content modulate amygdala-hippocampal connectivity in emotional retrieval. Neuron, 49(4), 631–8.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  180. Steinmetz, K. R., & Kensinger, E. A. (2013). The emotion-induced memory trade-off: More than an effect of overt attention? Memory and Cognition, 41(1), 69–81.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  181. St-Laurent, M., Abdi, H., Bondad, A., & Buchsbaum, B. R. (2014). Memory reactivation in healthy aging: Evidence of stimulus-specific dedifferentiation. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(12), 4175–4186. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3054-13.2014 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  182. Storbeck, J., & Clore, G. L. (2005). With sadness comes accuracy; with happiness, false memory: mood and the false memory effect. Psychol Sci, 16(10), 785–791. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01615.x PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  183. Talarico, J. M., & Rubin, D. C. (2003). Confidence, Not Consistency, Characterizes Flashbulb Memories. Psychological Science, 14(5), 455–461. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.02453 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  184. Talmi, D. (2013). Enhanced Emotional Memory: Cognitive and Neural Mechanisms. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(6), 430–436. doi: 10.1177/0963721413498893 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  185. Talmi, D., & McGarry, L. M. (2012). Accounting for immediate emotional memory enhancement. Journal of Memory and Language, 66(1), 93–108. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2011.07.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  186. Taylor, S. E. (1991). Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: The mobilizationminimization hypothesis. Psychological Review, 110, 67–85.Google Scholar
  187. Taylor, S. F., Liberzon, I., Fig, L. M., Decker, L. R., Minoshima, S., & Koeppe, R. A. (1998). The effect of emotional content on visual recognition memory: A PET activation study. NeuroImage, 8(2), 188–197. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0356 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  188. Teyler, T. J., & Rudy, J. W. (2007). The hippocampal indexing theory and episodic memory: Updating the index. Hippocampus, 17(12), 1158–1169. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20350 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  189. Todd, R. M., Talmi, D., Schmitz, T. W., Susskind, J., & Anderson, A. K. (2012). Psychophysical and neural evidence for emotion-enhanced perceptual vividness. J Neurosci, 32(33), 11201–11212. doi:  10.1523/jneurosci.0155-12.2012 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  190. Todd, R. M., Schmitz, T. W., Susskind, J., & Anderson, A. K. (2013). Shared neural substrates of emotionally enhanced perceptual and mnemonic vividness. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 40. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00040 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  191. Tse, C.-S., & Altarriba, J. (2009). The word concreteness effect occurs for positive, but not negative, emotion words in immediate serial recall. British Journal of Psychology, 100(1), 91–109.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  192. Tulving, E. (1983). Ecphoric process in episodic memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 361–370. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1983.0060
  193. Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic Memory: From Mind to Brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 1–25. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  194. Tulving, E., & Thomson, D. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval process in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80(5), 352–373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  195. Vaidya, C. J., Zhao, M., Desmond, J. E., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002). Evidence for cortical encoding specificity in episodic memory: Memory-induced re-activation of picture processing areas. Neuropsychologia, 40(12), 2136–2143. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00053-2 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  196. van de Ven, V., & Sack, A. T. (2013). Transcranial magnetic stimulation of visual cortex in memory: Cortical state, interference and reactivation of visual content in memory. Behavioural Brain Research, 236(1), 67–77.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  197. van den Honert, R. N., McCarthy, G., & Johnson, M. K. (2016). Reactivation during encoding supports the later discrimination of similar episodic memories. Hippocampus, 26(9), 1168–78. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22598 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  198. van Dongen, E. V., Takashima, A., Barth, M., Zapp, J., Schad, L. R., Paller, K. A., & Fernandez, G. (2012). Memory stabilization with targeted reactivation during human slow-wave sleep. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(26), 10575–10580. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1201072109 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  199. Vuilleumier, P., & Driver, J. (2007). Modulation of visual processing by attention and emotion: Windows on causal interactions between human brain regions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 362, 37–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  200. Wabnitz, P., Martens, U., & Neuner, F. (2016). Written threat: Electrophysiological evidence for an attention bias to affective words in social anxiety disorder. Cognition & Emotion, 30(3), 516–38. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2015.1019837 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  201. Wagner, U., Gais, S., & Born, J. (2001). Emotional memory formation is enhanced across sleep intervals with high amounts of rapid eye movement sleep. Learning and Memory, 8(2), 112–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  202. Waldhauser, G. T., Braun, V., & Hanslmayr, S. (2016). Episodic Memory Retrieval Functionally Relies on Very Rapid Reactivation of Sensory Information. The Journal of Neuroscience, 36(1), 251–260. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2101-15.2016 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  203. Walker, J. A., Low, K. A., Fletcher, M. A., Cohen, N. J., Gratton, G., & Fabiani, M. (2017). Hippocampal structure predicts cortical indices of reactivation of related items. Neuropsychologia, 95, 182–192. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.12.005 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  204. Wamsley, E. J., Tucker, M., Payne, J. D., Benavides, J. A., & Stickgold, R. (2010). Dreaming of a learning task is associated with enhanced sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Current Biology, 20(9), 850–855. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.03.027 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  205. Wang, B. (2013a). Gender difference in recognition memory for neutral and emotional faces. Memory, 21(8), 991–1003. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2013.771273 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  206. Wang, B. (2013b). Facial expression influences recognition memory for faces: Robust enhancement effect of fearful expression. Memory, 21(3), 301–14. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2012.725740 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  207. Wang, B. (2014). Effect of time delay on recognition memory for pictures: The modulatory role of emotion. PLoS ONE, 9(6), e100238.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  208. Wang, B., & Fu, X. (2011). Time course of effects of emotion on item memory and source memory for Chinese words. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 95(4), 415–424.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  209. Weymar, M., Löw, A., Melzig, C. A., & Hamm, A. O. (2009). Enhanced long-term recollection for emotional pictures: Evidence from high-density ERPs. Psychophysiology, 46, 1220–1207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  210. Weymar, M., Löw, A., & Hamm, A. O. (2011). Emotional memories are resilient to time: Evidence from the parietal ERP old/new effect. Human Brain Mapping, 32(4), 632–40. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21051 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  211. Whalley, M. G., Kroes, M. C., Huntley, Z., Rugg, M. D., Davis, S. W., & Brewin, C. R. (2013). An fMRI investigation of posttraumatic flashbacks. Brain and Cognition, 81(1), 151–159. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2012.10.002 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  212. Wheeler, M. E., Petersen, S. E., & Buckner, R. L. (2000). Memory’s echo: Vivid remembering reactivates sensory-specific cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(20), 11125–11129. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.20.11125 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  213. Wilson, M. A., & McNaughton, B. L. (1994). Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble memories during sleep. Science, 265(5172), 676–679.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  214. Wing, E. A., Ritchey, M., & Cabeza, R. (2015). Reinstatement of Individual Past Events Revealed by the Similarity of Distributed Activation Patterns during Encoding and Retrieval. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(4), 679–691. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00740 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  215. Xie, W., & Zhang, W. (2016). Negative emotion boosts quality of visual working memory representation. Emotion, 16(5), 760–774. doi: 10.1037/emo0000159 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  216. Yonelinas, A. P., & Ritchey, M. (2015). The slow forgetting of emotional episodic memories: An emotional binding account. Trends in Cognitive Science, 19(5), 259–267. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.02.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  217. Zeithamova, D., Dominick, A. L., & Preston, A. R. (2012). Hippocampal and ventral medial prefrontal activation during retrieval-mediated learning supports novel inference. Neuron, 75(1), 168–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.05.010 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  • Holly J. Bowen
    • 1
  • Sarah M. Kark
    • 1
  • Elizabeth A. Kensinger
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyBoston CollegeChestnut HillUSA

Personalised recommendations