Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

, Volume 22, Issue 3, pp 844–849 | Cite as

Mentalizing or submentalizing in a communication task? Evidence from autism and a camera control

  • Idalmis Santiesteban
  • Punit Shah
  • Sarah White
  • Geoffrey Bird
  • Cecilia Heyes
Brief Report

Abstract

In the director task (DT), participants are instructed to move objects within a grid of shelves while ignoring those objects that cannot be seen by a human figure, the “director,” located beyond the shelves. It is widely assumed that, since they are explicitly instructed to do, participants use mentalizing in this communicative task; they represent what the director can see, and therefore the DT provides important information about how and when mentalizing is used in adult life. We tested this view against a “submentalizing” hypothesis suggesting that DT performance depends on object-centered spatial coding, without mentalizing. As predicted by the submentalizing account, we found that DT performance was unchanged when the director was replaced by an inanimate object, a camera, and that participants with autism spectrum disorders were unimpaired, relative to matched control participants, in both the director and camera conditions. In combination with recent critical analyses of “implicit mentalizing,” these findings support the view that adults use mentalizing sparingly in psychological experiments and in everyday life.

Keywords

Perspective-taking Mentalizing Submentalizing Object-centered spatial coding Director task Autism 

Notes

Author note

The authors thank Iroise Dumontheil for providing helpful feedback on an earlier version of the manuscript. This work was supported by an Economic and Social Research Council studentship [ES/H013504/1] awarded to I.S.

Supplementary material

13423_2014_716_MOESM1_ESM.pdf (96 kb)
ESM 1 (PDF 96 kb)

References

  1. Aichhorn, M., Perner, J., Kronbichler, M., Staffen, W., & Ladurner, G. (2006). Do visual perspective tasks need theory of mind? NeuroImage, 30, 1059–1068. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.10.026 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Apperly, I. A., Carroll, D. J., Samson, D., Humphreys, G. W., Qureshi, A., & Moffitt, G. (2010). Why are there limits on theory of mind use? Evidence from adults’ ability to follow instructions from an ignorant speaker. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 1201–1217. doi: 10.1080/17470210903281582 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 5–17. doi: 10.1023/A:1005653411471 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Begeer, S., Malle, B. F., Nieuwland, M. S., & Keysar, B. (2010). Using theory of mind to represent and take part in social interactions: Comparing individuals with high-functioning autism and typically developing controls. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 104–122. doi: 10.1080/17405620903024263 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., Schütz-Bosbacck, S., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2011). How “social” is the social Simon effect? Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 84. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00084 CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Dumontheil, I., Apperly, I. A., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2010). Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence. Developmental Science, 13, 331–338. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00888.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Dumontheil, I., Küster, O., Apperly, I. A., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2010). Taking perspective into account in a communicative task. NeuroImage, 52, 1574–1583. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.056 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Frith, U. (2012). Why we need cognitive explanations of autism. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 2073–2092. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.697178 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Happé, F. G. E. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 129–154. doi: 10.1007/BF02172093 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Heyes, C. M. (2014). Submentalizing: I’m not really reading your mind. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 131–143. doi: 10.1177/1745691613518076 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Heyes, C. M., & Frith, C. D. (2014). The cultural evolution of mind reading. Science, 344, 1243091. doi: 10.1126/science.1243091 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Keysar, B., Barr, D. J., Balin, J. A., & Brauner, J. S. (2000). Taking perspective in conversation: The role of mutual knowledge in comprehension. Psychological Science, 11, 32–38. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00211 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Keysar, B., Lin, S., & Barr, D. J. (2003). Limits on theory of mind use in adults. Cognition, 89, 25–41. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00064-7 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Jr., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., . . . Rutter, M. (2000). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223. doi: 10.1023/A:1005592401947
  15. Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E. A., & Nieuwenhuis, S. (2004). The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science, 306, 443–447. doi: 10.1126/science.1100301 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Samson, D., Apperly, I. A., Braithwaite, J. J., Andrews, B. J., & Bodley Scott, S. E. (2010). Seeing it their way: Evidence for rapid and involuntary computation of what other people see. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1255–1266. doi: 10.1037/a0018729 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Santiesteban, I., Catmur, C., Coughlan Hopkins, S., Bird, G., & Heyes, C. (2014). Avatars and arrows: Implicit mentalizing or domain-general processing? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 929–937. doi: 10.1037/a0035175 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Schilbach, L., Timmermans, B., Reddy, V., Costall, A., Bente, G., Schlicht, T., & Vogeley, K. (2013). Toward a second-person neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 393–414. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12000660 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Schwarzkopf, S., Schilbach, L., Vogeley, K., & Timmermans, B. (2014). Making it explicit makes a difference: Evidence for a dissociation of spontaneous and intentional level 1 perspective taking in high-functioning autism. Cognition, 131, 345–354. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.02.003 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., & Frith, U. (2009). Mindblind eyes: An absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger syndrome. Science, 325, 883–885. doi: 10.1126/science.1176170 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Surtees, A., Apperly, I., & Samson, D. (2013). Similarities and differences in visual and spatial perspective-taking processes. Cognition, 129, 426–438. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.06.00 CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler adult intelligence scale (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp.Google Scholar
  23. Wu, S., & Keysar, B. (2007). The effect of culture on perspective taking. Psychological Science, 18, 600–606. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01946.x CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Idalmis Santiesteban
    • 1
  • Punit Shah
    • 1
    • 2
  • Sarah White
    • 3
  • Geoffrey Bird
    • 2
    • 3
  • Cecilia Heyes
    • 4
  1. 1.Department of Psychological Sciences, BirkbeckUniversity of LondonLondonUK
  2. 2.MRC Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry CentreInstitute of Psychiatry, King’s College LondonLondonUK
  3. 3.Institute of Cognitive NeuroscienceUniversity College LondonLondonUK
  4. 4.All Souls CollegeUniversity of OxfordOxfordUK

Personalised recommendations